Pursuing personal goals is a ubiquitous aspect of human nature, so it is no wonder that psychology researchers have investigated how goal pursuit occurs. But to what extent do the ways that researchers study goal pursuit shape the answers they obtain? Past research has used various definitions of personal goals, along with numerous constructs that are similar to goals (e.g., personal projects, personal strivings). Conclusions from studies using these distinct constructs have been used to generalize to goal pursuit more broadly, assuming that the specific definition provided to participants, or the way that the list of personal goals is elicited, does not matter. We test this assumption directly by experimentally examining whether different ways of asking participants about their goals would elicit different goals and impact reports of goal progress.
Goal Elicitation and the Jangle Problem
Research on personal goals and other constructs that can be considered similar to goals, such as personal projects (Little, 1983), has led to a potential jangle fallacy (Kelley, 1927), which refers to the erroneous belief that constructs are theoretically different just because they have different names. Goals have been defined as “a cognitive representation of a desired end state that a person is committed to attain” (Milyavskaya & Werner, 2018, p. 163). Personal projects, on the other hand, are defined as “a set of interrelated acts extending over time, which is intended to maintain or attain a state of affairs foreseen by the individual” (Little, 1983, p. 276). Based on these definitions, goals are what people want, and personal projects are what people do (for a more thorough discussion of these distinctions and arguments, see Little, 2007, pp. 36–38). The lines between the two, however, are often blurred, and many researchers use the terms interchangeably (e.g., a book on personal projects has many chapters focusing on personal goals; Little et al., 2007).
Beyond the conceptual definitions, many similarities can be found in the methods used to study these constructs. In both cases, participants are asked to think of the personal projects or personal goals that they are currently pursuing, engaged in, or are planning to pursue, and are asked to name between 3–15 goals/projects (e.g., Koestner et al., 2002; Little & Gee, 2007). Each goal or project is then rated on several characteristics. Indeed, some researchers use personal projects methodology to examine what they term personal goals (e.g., Salmela-Aro et al., 2007; Sheldon & Kasser, 1998). For example, Sheldon and Kasser (1998) report using Little’s (1983) personal projects methodology to elicit goals for a study on personal goals and goal progress. In some cases, researchers do not even report the exact definition or wording that is used to elicit goals from participants, stating simply that participants were asked to name a certain number of personal goals (e.g., Riediger & Freund, 2004). Similarly, when reviewing past literature, authors typically combine findings from studies using personal project definitions with goal definitions (and include both in meta-analyses, e.g., Klug & Maier, 2015). There is currently no evidence, however, that supports such interchangeable use of these constructs. Is it the case that goals and personal projects, despite having different names and representing theoretically different constructs, are actually perceived in the same way by study participants? Or does the way researchers elicit goals from participants impact participants’ answers, and ultimately the conclusions that can be drawn from the research?
Furthermore, both definitions of goals and personal projects are specific definitions derived from researchers’ conceptualizations of goals. But how does this match up with what laypeople think about when asked about their goals? Do people spontaneously think of lofty, long-term goals? Or do they think about what they have to do in the near term? People’s lay understanding of what is meant by goals is likely to color their answers to this question, but this has not been investigated. Specifically, does the detailed description frequently used in research on personal goals result in people providing a different set of goals than what they would otherwise think of and pursue in their daily life? We know that simply setting goals can lead to greater progress on those goals (Norcross et al., 2002); are studies where researchers provide a detailed description of goals inherently manipulating something by leading participants to write down more goals, or different goals that they would otherwise be thinking about, thereby getting them to ‘set’ new goals and increasing goal attainment? If research on goal pursuit hopes to be relevant to people’s actual day-to-day goal pursuit, it needs to ensure that the way researchers ask about goals does not inadvertently lead participants to set more, or different goals than they would have otherwise.
Goal Characteristics
Each goal that a person pursues can be described along several dimensions that can characterize the nature of the goal (Austin & Vancouver, 1996). Here, we focused on four that have been consistently linked to goal progress, and thus are likely to be of particular interest to goal researchers: difficulty, specificity, commitment, and motivational quality.
Goal difficulty represents the standard of attainment, or how much effort would be required for the individual to accomplish the goal (Locke & Latham, 1990). Relatedly, goal specificity corresponds to the level of abstraction of a goal (Carver & Scheier, 1982). Specific goals (sometimes referred to as concrete goals; Fujita & MacGregor, 2012), in contrast to broad/abstract goals, frequently have an identifiable end-point, and require relatively fewer, concrete actions to attain. Personal projects have been proposed to be more context-dependent than personal goals, and focus specifically on actions carried out in the service of goals (Little, 2007, p. 37). If this theoretical distinction exists, participants should view their personal projects as more concrete and grounded than the likely loftier and aspirational goals, which may be less specific and/or more difficult. In a similar vein, participants asked about personal projects should indicate more projects (since they are expected to be smaller, more specific actions that are easier to accomplish).
Commitment can be defined as determination (Locke & Latham, 1990), or “how long an individual is willing to strive for a specific goal” (Austin & Vancouver, 1996). Some level of commitment distinguishes goals from fantasies or wishes (Gollwitzer, 1990). And greater goal commitment has been linked to greater effort, persistence, and performance (Koestner et al., 2002). Given that commitment is an integral aspect of goals, we expected that when participants are asked to spontaneously think of personal goals (without any definitions or further prompts), they would be more likely to think of those goals to which they are most committed, such that commitment would be highest in that condition (compared to, for example, thinking about all the personal projects that they are currently pursuing).
Finally, motivational quality refers to the reasons why the person is pursuing the goal. Specifically, people can pursue a goal because it is interesting, enjoyable, feels like a part of the self, or is personally important (autonomous motivation), but also because of external or internal pressures (e.g., rewards, fear of negative consequences, guilt; termed controlled motivation; Ryan & Deci, 2017). More autonomous goals may be more closely integrated with other goals (Sheldon, 2014), and thus may be more chronically accessible and easier to bring to mind. When asked in an open-ended manner about their goals, people may thus be most likely to think about goals that are more autonomous (compared to when they are asked to consider all the specific projects or goals that they are currently pursuing).
In addition to these four characteristics, we examined goal progress and attainment. Progress concerns the amount of movement towards the desired end state (Carver & Scheier, 1982), whereas attainment is a dichotomous judgment of whether the goal was accomplished; this allows us to compute the number of goals that have been attained over a specific time period. If participants setting open-ended goals are indeed setting more abstract, difficult goals, they could be expected to attain fewer of these goals, despite subjectively feeling that they are generally making good progress on their goals. In contrast, personal projects, which focus on smaller, in-the-moment concerns, may elicit goals that can be accomplished over a shorter time frame, such that more of them could be completed within the six weeks of the current study.
Present Study
The present study examined whether instructions used to elicit goals/personal projects affect (1) the number of goals participants report; (2) the characteristics of the goals they report; and (3) subsequent self-reported progress on those goals. We hypothesized that asking about personal projects would elicit more goals and result in greater goal attainment, but that asking about goals in an open-ended manner would result in less specific, more difficult goals, that are greater in commitment and autonomous motivation. We had also pre-registered (https://osf.io/fjm7p) a secondary aim of this project: to examine to what extent self-reported Big Five personality traits correlated with individual differences in reports of goal characteristics and goal progress. Because this is beyond the scope of the current work (which is concerned with elicitation methods), we present these findings in the Supplementary Materials.
All hypotheses and planned analyses were pre-registered after data from wave 1 was collected, but before any of the data was cleaned or analyzed (see https://osf.io/fjm7p). Given the likelihood of null effects (i.e., it may not matter how we ask about goals), we supplemented frequentist analyses with Bayesian analyses that allow us to specify our confidence in the effects (null or otherwise). We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions, all manipulations, and all measures (either in the text or on OSF). All materials, data, syntax, and full output from the analyses can be found in the Supplementary Materials1.
Method
Participants and Procedure
Ethical approval was obtained from the university REB. Participants were 285 undergraduate students (MAge = 20.12, SD = 3.89, 76% female) who completed the study for course credit. We aimed to collect at least 160 participants (to have 80% power to detect a medium effect size, f = .25, in a one-way ANOVA with three conditions), but planned to continue collecting data for the first six weeks of the winter 2020 semester (until reading week) to have power to detect smaller effects. We ended data collection with 289 participants; 4 were removed: 1 withdrew from the study; 3 others were removed because they were outliers on the number of goals indicated, each with over 20 goals (more than 3 SD from the mean, and clear outliers on visual inspection of the histogram in the Supplementary Materials). Analyses were conducted with 285 participants (analyses with the outliers retained were essentially the same, and are reported in the Supplementary Materials).
Participants came into the lab and were randomly assigned to one of three goal elicitation conditions. They received an instructions sheet asking them to name all their current goals/personal projects in one of three ways (see the Supplementary Materials for exact wording):
-
Using the personal projects methodology, including a definition of personal projects (from Little & Gee, 2007).
-
Asking about personal goals, with a definition most commonly used in the personal goal literature (e.g., Koestner et al., 2008; Werner et al., 2016).
-
In an open-ended manner, without providing a definition of goals (“Everyone pursues goals. Please think of the goals you are currently pursuing and write them below.”).
A blank sheet was provided to participants to write down their goals/projects (there were no lines on the sheet, to avoid inadvertently suggesting to participants how many they ‘should’ be writing based on the number of blank spaces). After manually writing down all their goals/projects, participants entered their goals/projects one at a time into a computerized survey, rated each goal/project on several characteristics, and completed personality questionnaires (see OSF for full survey). Six weeks later, in an optional online survey, 122 participants (42%) reported their progress and attainment for each of the goals/projects2. There were no differences in the number of goals, the assigned conditions, or the goal characteristics between participants who completed the follow-up versus those who did not (see Supplementary Materials). A sensitivity analysis showed 80% power to detect an effect size of f = .18 (η2 = .03) in time 1 data, and f = .28 (η2 = .07) in the follow-up.
Measures
Number of Goals
Participants were given a blank page to write down as many personal goals/projects as they wanted. They were then asked to count the number of goals written and enter this number into the electronic survey; this was checked manually by research assistants, and when there was a discrepancy, the number written on the paper prevailed.
Goal Characteristics
For each nominated goal/project, participants rated each of the following characteristics using one face-valid item: specificity (“How would you define the current goal/project you are pursuing?” 1 = very specific; 7 = very broad); difficulty (“I think it will be difficult for me to reach this goal.”) and commitment (“I feel that I am committed to this goal.”), both scales 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree. For motivation, three items per goal assessed autonomous motivation (one item each for intrinsic, identified, and integrated), and two items assessed controlled motivation (one item each for external and introjected, Koestner et al., 2008). All items were rated on a scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Average specificity, difficulty, commitment, and autonomous and controlled motivation were calculated for each person by averaging across all of the person’s goals/projects.
Goal Progress and Attainment
This was assessed at the online follow-up 6 weeks after the initial in-lab study, and measured in two ways. First, for each goal/project, participants reported goal progress on one item (“I have made a lot of progress toward this goal”), rated on a 1 (strongly disagree) – 7 (strongly agree) scale. The average across all goals/projects comprised the continuous measure of goal progress. Second, for each goal/project, participants were asked whether they achieved the goal/project, were still pursuing it, abandoned it, changed or adjusted it, or were unsure; the total number of achieved goals/projects was counted.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
After removing the three outliers who listed more than 20 goals, the average number of goals amongst participants was 7.5 (SD = 3.5). The most frequent responses were either 5 (15.3%), 6 (14.6%), or 7 (10.4%) goals. As can be seen in Table 1, these goals were rated as moderately difficult (higher than the midpoint of 4), and more specific rather than broad.
Participants also reported being very committed to their goals. Motivation for goals was relatively more autonomous than controlled. To examine variability across goals, we computed the intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) for each variable applied to goals nested within people; the ICC represents the proportion of the variance at the person level, with the rest of the variance representing the variance due to the goals themselves or to error. As in past research (e.g., Holding et al., 2017; Milyavskaya & Inzlicht, 2017; Nurmi et al., 2009; Werner et al., 2016), approximately 71 to 75% of the variance in goal characteristics and 88% of the variance in goal progress was at the goal level.
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Pearson Correlations With 95% Confidence Intervals of All Measured Study Variables (Collapsed Across Condition)
Variable | M | SD | ICC | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Number of goals | 7.48 | 3.49 | |||||||||
2. Difficulty | 4.55 | 1.23 | .27 | -.15** [-.26, -.04] |
|||||||
3. Specificitya | 3.16 | 1.19 | .25 | .07 [-.05, .18] |
.12* [.00, .23] |
||||||
4. Commitment | 5.63 | 0.87 | .27 | .09 [-.03, .20] |
-.16** [-.28, -.05] |
-.26*** [-.37, -.15] |
|||||
5. Autonomous Motivation | 5.24 | 0.82 | .26 | -.02 [-.13, .10] |
.07 [-.04, .19] |
.03 [-.09, .14] |
.45*** [.35, .54] |
||||
6. Controlled Motivation | 3.50 | 1.08 | .29 | .00 [-.11, .12] |
.19** [.07, .30] |
.08 [-.04, .20] |
-.19*** [-.30, -.08] |
-.13* [-.25, -.02] |
|||
7. Progress | 4.66 | 0.93 | .12 | -.06 [-.24, .12] |
-.22* [-.38, -.04] |
-.32*** [-.47, -.15] |
.34*** [.17, .49] |
.04 [-.14, .21] |
-.09 [-.26, .09] |
||
8. Number attained | 1.10 | 1.70 | .47*** [.32, .60] |
-.19* [-.36, -.02] |
.01 [-.17, .19] |
.00 [-.18, .18] |
-.28** [-.43, -.10] |
.26** [.09, .42] |
.23* [.05, .39] |
||
9. Proportion attained | 0.14 | 0.17 | .17 [-.01, .34] |
-.16 [-.33, .02] |
-.08 [-.26, .10] |
-.02 [-.19, .16] |
-.29*** [-.45, -.12] |
.18* [.00, .35] |
.15 [-.03, .32] |
.87*** [.82, .91] |
Note. ICC = intraclass correlation (proportion of variance on between-person level, calculated by dividing the intercept variance by the total variance from empty mixed models; see OSF for calculations); N = 285 for correlations among goal characteristics, N = 120 for correlations with goal progress and attainment.
aHigher numbers = less specific (more broad).
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Effects of Goal Elicitation
We first examined differences across the three conditions on the number of goals that participants set (see Table 2 for all comparisons across condition). As hypothesized, participants reported more goals in the personal projects condition than in the open-ended or personal goal conditions. A series of ANOVAs3 were conducted with difficulty, specificity, commitment, and autonomous and controlled motivation as dependent variables. Contrary to pre-registered hypotheses, the open-ended goal condition did not elicit less specific goals, more difficult goals (than the detailed goals condition), or goals that were greater in commitment and autonomous motivation. There was a difference across conditions in difficulty, such that personal projects were rated as less difficult than goals entered in both the detailed and open-ended conditions. Finally, contrary to expectations, there were no differences across conditions in goal progress or goal attainment.
Table 2
Differences in Key Goal Variables Across Elicitation Conditions
Dependent Variable | Detailed goals N = 97 |
Open goals N = 95 |
Personal projects N = 93 |
ANOVA results | η2 [90% CI] | BF10 | BF01 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Number of goals | 6.56 (2.98)a | 7.00 (3.17)a | 8.92 (3.86)b | F(2, 284) = 13.26, p < .001 | .086 [.04, .14] | 4983.86 | |
Difficulty | 4.90 (1.07)a | 4.68 (1.19)a | 4.06 (1.28)b | F(2, 284) = 12.78, p < .001 | .083[.04, .13] | 3191.03 | |
Specificity | 3.24 (1.31) | 3.10 (1.13) | 3.12 (1.11) | F(2, 284) = .35, p = .708 | .002 [.00, .01] | 19.40 | |
Commitment | 5.48 (.88)a | 5.67 (.82)ab | 5.75 (.90)b | F(2, 284) = 2.42, p = .091 | .017 [.00, .04] | 2.98 | |
Autonomous Motivation | 5.13 (.81) | 5.33 (.83) | 5.25 (.81) | F(2, 284) = 1.39, p = .250 | .010[.00, .03] | 7.52 | |
Controlled Motivation | 3.64 (1.06) | 3.51 (1.13) | 3.35 (1.03) | F(2, 284) = 1.70, p = .185 | .012 [.00, .04] | 5.72 | |
Progress | 4.50 (.86) | 4.73 (1.01) | 4.77 (.92) | F(2, 119) = 1.00, p = .372 | .017 [.00, .06] | 5.52 | |
Number attained | .88 (1.37) | 1.10 (1.68) | 1.34 (2.04) | F(2, 119) = .73, p = .485 | .012 [.00, .05] | 6.91 | |
Proportion attained | .12 (.16) | .15 (.18) | .14 (.18) | F(2, 119) = .35, p = .702 | .006 [.00, .03] | 9.39 |
Note. Bolded values are statistically significant at p < .05. Different superscripts in the same row indicate significantly different values. BFs can be interpreted as follows: 1–3 = weak evidence, 3–10 = moderate evidence, 10+ = strong evidence (Schönbrodt & Wagenmakers, 2018). 90% CIs are reported for η2 because it is one-sided (since it cannot be below 0, and is equivalent to a 95% CI for a Cohen’s d for the same test; see Lakens, 2014, for more details).
To check the robustness of these findings we conducted follow-up Bayesian analyses using JASP software4. We report all computed Bayes Factors (BFs) in Table 2. As can be seen, the results for number of goals and goal difficulty indicated that the model with elicitation condition entered as a predictor is thousands of times better than the null, strongly suggesting that there were differences across conditions for these variables. For specificity, the null is almost 20 times better, suggesting no effects of condition. For the other dependent variables, however, the results were somewhat less conclusive, suggesting moderate support.
We next examined the variability of goal characteristics across the goals, both by looking at the intraclass correlations (ICCs), and by computing the standard deviation for each characteristic across each person’s goals, separately for each condition. Full results are presented in the Supplementary Materials, but essentially suggest that although some differences in ICCs can be seen across conditions, goal elicitation did not significantly affect variability in any of the goal characteristics or goal progress across goals.
Additional Exploratory Analyses
Finally, in exploratory (non pre-registered) analyses, we examined whether responses are different for the first three or first four reported goals, compared to the average from all the goals reported, on each goal characteristic and goal attainment (using paired t-tests). This was done to examine the potential effects of asking participants to only list three or four goals, as is common in many studies of goal pursuit (e.g., Holding et al., 2019; Milyavskaya et al., 2015). As seen in Table 3, there were differences in specificity, commitment, controlled motivation, and progress. Supplementary analyses found no interaction with condition (except for commitment) – that is, the first few goals were different from the average of all the goals in the same way across conditions. Overall, these analyses showed that the first few goals that a person reports are more specific, higher on commitment, more controlled (but not more or less autonomous), and slightly more difficult (only for the first three, not the first four). People also report (slightly) more progress on the first few goals5.
Table 3
Differences in Goal Ratings Between all the Goals Compared to Either the First Three or First Four Goals (From Repeated-Samples t-Tests)
Dependent Variable | First 3 M (SD) |
First 3 vs overall N = 281 |
First 4 M (SD) |
First 4 vs overall N = 255 |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Overall M (SD) |
t | p | D [95% CI] |
Overall M (SD) |
t | p | D [95% CI] |
|||
Difficulty | 4.49 (1.40) | 4.58 (1.21) | -2.04 | .043 | .12 [.00, 24] |
4.50 (1.24) | 4.55 (1.15) | 1.28 | .201 | .08 [-.04, .20] |
Specificity | 2.98 (1.35) | 3.17 (1.18) | 3.87 | < .001 | .23 [.11, .35] |
3.05 (1.26) | 3.18 (1.17) | 3.21 | .001 | .20 [.08, .33] |
Commitment | 5.77 (.93) | 5.63 (.86) | 4.64 | < .001 | .28 [.16, 40] |
5.75 (.89) | 5.65 (.83) | 3.69 | < .001 | .23 [.11, .36] |
Autonomous motivation | 5.21 (.96) | 5.23 (.82) | -.80 | .422 | .05 [-.07, .17] |
5.17 (.89) | 5.21 (.80) | -1.46 | .143 | .09 [-.03, .22] |
Controlled motivation | 3.63 (1.22) | 3.52 (1.08) | 2.72 | .007 | .16 [.04, .28] |
3.63 (1.15) | 3.54 (1.07) | 2.40 | .017 | .15 [.03, .27] |
Progress (N = 118 & N = 108) | 4.85 (1.10) | 4.65 (.93) | 3.46 | .001 | .32 [.13; .50] |
4.79 (.96) | 4.64 (.90) | 3.29 | .001 | .32 [.12, .51] |
Note. Bolded values are statistically significant at p < .05.
Discussion
The present study examined whether the way that goals are elicited makes a difference in the number and characteristics of goals that are reported, and in subsequent goal progress. In line with our hypothesis, we found that asking about personal projects led participants to report more goals than asking about personal goals using either the specific or open-ended formulation. These goals were also rated as less difficult, and participants in the personal project condition reported more commitment for their goals than the detailed goals condition (but not the open-ended goals). There were no other differences across conditions. Contrary to our hypotheses, the open-ended goal condition did not elicit less specific goals, and participants did not report more autonomous motivation or greater commitment for these goals. There were also no differences across condition in goal progress or goal attainment. Further Bayesian analyses suggested moderate support for the null hypotheses. Altogether, these findings suggest that these elicitation methods appear to result in mostly similar goals on most characteristics (other than difficulty). Unless difficulty or number of goals are of particular interest, research can likely continue to combine findings and draw conclusions across studies using different elicitations methodologies.
Overall, we found that participants reported a greater number of goals, which they rated as less difficult, in the personal projects condition. This fits with the theoretical understanding of personal projects as something that people are currently working on, which may include daily tasks that would not typically qualify as goals. It may also be that providing examples, and a longer definition, led participants to think more broadly about their day-to-day pursuits and record these as personal projects, even though they would not normally meet the definition of goals. We also did not examine the timescale of the goals that participants reported – that is, one key difference may have been in the extent to which participants in the different conditions set short-term versus long-term goals. However, given that more abstract goals typically take longer to attain, and conversely shorter-term goals could be expected to be more specific, combined with the lack of differences in specificity found the current study, it suggests that personal goals and personal projects do not elicit goals that are pursued on different timescales. Future research, however, needs to examine this more closely.
There were no differences between providing a detailed definition of goals and simply asking participants to think about their personal goals, which suggests that the detailed definition approximates what people generally think of goals. This is encouraging, as researchers’ definitions of a construct do not necessarily parallel lay understandings of this construct. In the case of goals, however, it seems that the definition frequently provided by researchers leads people (or at least undergraduate students) to think of the same thing as if they were simply asked to think of goals more generally. Note, however, that since the Bayes analyses showed only weak to moderate support for the null hypothesis (that there are truly no differences across conditions), further research should obtain additional evidence until a strong conclusion can be drawn, preferably with data from different populations.
One key difference across the conditions was the length of the descriptions of the goals provided to participants, with 196 words (including concrete examples) in the personal project condition, 92 words in the personal goals condition, and 41 words in the open answer condition. Although this may at first appear to be a confound, these descriptions represent the actual way in which these types of goals are typically elicited in the literature; any attempts at making the description the same length would be a departure from the elicitation procedure that is typically used to ask about that type of goal. Since the main aim of our study was to compare existing elicitation procedures, we chose to use existing wording rather than artificially shorten or lengthen it to equate them on length. Our results, showing that there are very few differences in the goals provided in response to these conditions, suggests that length of the text may not matter much either.
In exploratory analyses examining differences between variables computed across all goals and the first three or four goals that a person reported, we did find some differences. This suggests that contrary to the personal project elicitation, which typically asks for 10–15 projects (Little & Gee, 2007), asking about three or four goals as is typically done in goal research (e.g., Holding et al., 2017, 2019; Werner et al., 2016) results in a focus on goals that are particularly important and more likely to be attained. This is potentially important for generalizing to goal pursuit more broadly. However, the differences were relatively small (less than 0.2 on a 1–7 scale); it remains to be seen whether these differences actually affect inferences that can be drawn from research using different elicitations. Additionally, the average number of goals that participants indicated was 7.5, suggesting that the practice of asking participants to list 10–15 projects may artificially lead participants to report goals that they do not truly care about. The average number of goals reported in our study is similar to other research where participants were allowed to indicate as many or as few idiographic goals as they wanted (Monzani et al., 2015).
The present research focused on three different elicitations; however, researchers have conceptualized other similar constructs including personal concerns (Klinger & Cox, 2004), life tasks (Cantor et al., 1987), and personal strivings (Emmons, 1992). Research focusing on these distinct constructs all elicit and assess goals in slightly different ways (Lloyd et al., 2019; for a review see Negru, 2011). The present study, however, suggests that for at least some of these, there are limited differences in the resulting data. It may thus be that despite conceptual and methodological differences, a continued reliance on these different constructs and terminology only serves to fracture the literature, promulgating a further jingle jangle problem in research that ultimately assesses practically indistinguishable constructs. More efforts are needed to theoretically and empirically integrate these various constructs to arrive at a better understanding of goal pursuit.
Limitations
This study was conducted with first and second-year undergraduate students on a university campus in a western country; our sample is thus young and predominantly female (76%), white (57%), and educated. It is thus unknown if these results would generalize to other populations. Much of the research on goal pursuit has been conducted with undergraduates (Koestner et al., 2008; Milyavskaya et al., 2015; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999), although there is also a sizeable literature on goal pursuit in adolescents (Massey et al., 2008) and older adults (e.g., Bailly et al., 2012; Riediger et al., 2005). A second limitation is the high dropout rate (58%) and the timing of the follow-up, which coincided with closures of the university and of new restrictions due to the COVID-19 pandemic. This may have affected the drop-out rate and very likely altered goal pursuit (see Footnote 1). Future research could examine how goal pursuit changes in the face of unforeseen or unusual circumstances. In this study, the results for goal progress and attainment should thus be interpreted with caution. Furthermore, goal progress was only assessed at the follow-up, and not at baseline, so that we did not examine change in progress over time. Another limitation of the research concerns the limited number of goal characteristics examined in the present study. It may be that our conditions did elicit goals that differed in other ways that were not assessed here, such as time scale, urgency, or enjoyment.
A final limitation is that all of the goal characteristics were assessed with single items (one per characteristic for each goal), and aggregated across goals. Although this is the standard for research in both personal projects and goal pursuit research (Little & Gee, 2007; Sheldon & Kasser, 1998), and the items are all face valid, this is still an issue as far as measurement is concerned. Perhaps more importantly, since most of the variation in goal characteristics was on the goal level, it is unclear how useful or meaningful it is to aggregate them across goals (see Lüdtke & Trautwein, 2007). That is, although personal project research explicitly treats such projects as a level of personality (Little, 1989), and extensive research has examined goal pursuit as an individual difference (e.g., Sheldon & Elliot, 1999), goal pursuit is first and foremost goal-specific (see Milyavskaya & Werner, 2018). Aggregating such goal-specific characteristics on a between-person level can result in unreliable measurement (since there is little shared variance on the between-person level) among items. Increasing the number of items per characteristic, however, would not help with this problem (although assessing more goals can; Lüdtke & Trautwein, 2007). Overall, attempting to investigate individual differences, while forgetting the extensive within-person variability, can hamper efforts that seek to better understand personal goal pursuit.
Conclusions
This study found that although using the personal project methodology elicited more goals and less difficult goals than asking about personal goals, there were no differences in other goal characteristics. This suggests that how participants are asked about their goals may not matter very much, particularly in studies that only ask participants about three or four goals. Contrary to our hypotheses, results also suggest that both personal goal and personal project definitions align with lay-person views of goals, as both lead to similar reported goals. This suggests that researchers can probably draw similar conclusions from studies that ask about goal pursuit in different ways, providing further confidence in our ability to build a more unified field of research on personal goal pursuit.