State of the Art Review [Invited Paper]

Personality and Social Relationships: What Do We Know and Where Do We Go

Mitja D. Back*1, Susan Branje2, Paul W. Eastwick3, Lauren J. Human4, Lars Penke5, Gentiana Sadikaj6, Richard B. Slatcher7, Isabel Thielmann8, Maarten H. W. van Zalk9, Cornelia Wrzus10

Personality Science, 2023, Vol. 4, Article e7505, https://doi.org/10.5964/ps.7505

Received: 2021-09-15. Accepted: 2022-05-12. Published (VoR): 2023-01-13.

Handling Editor: Marco Perugini, University of Milan-Bicocca, Milan, Italy

Reviewing: Round 1 - Anonymous #1; Anonymous #2. No open reviews are available.

*Corresponding author at: University of Münster, Institute of Psychology, Fliednerstr. 21, 48149 Münster, Germany. E-mail: mitja.back@wwu.de

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Personality and social relationships influence each other in multiple and consequential ways. To understand how people differ from each other in their personality and social behavior, how these differences develop, and how this affects further life outcomes, we need to better understand the interplay of personality and social relationships. Here, we provide an integrative overview on personality-relationship research across relationship types (everyday encounters, friendships, romantic, and family relationships), and personality characteristics. We summarize the state of research on (a) how much relationship aspects vary across actors, partners, and actor-partner relations, (b) which personality characteristics predict these variance components (i.e. actor, partner, and relationship effects), and (c) how social relationships work as contexts for personality development. Following an integrative process framework, key open questions are discussed concerning the processes that underlie personality-relationship and relationship-personality effects. We conclude with a call for conceptual integration, methodological expansion, and collaborative action.

Keywords: personality, social relationships, romantic relationships, family relationships, friendships, peer relations, social interaction processes, interpersonal perception

Relevance Statement

Personality and relationships influence each other in manifold ways; they cannot be understood in isolation. This paper summarizes the state of the art, provides a common framework for the future of science of personality and social relationships.

Key Insights

  • Emotional Stability, Communion, and Self-Control relate to getting along.

  • Agency and Sociability predict getting ahead.

  • Relationship variance is the largest but least understood variance component.

  • Evidence for effects of relationships on personality development is mixed.

  • More comprehensive and integrative research on underlying processes needed.

The rich diversity in which individuals differ from each other cannot be understood without a detailed consideration of how people interact and bond with others. The science of personality, thus, requires a close consideration of social relationships. This regards all three key tasks personality science faces (see Mõttus et al., 2020): Description, explanation and prediction. To describe personality, one needs to delineate the ways people act, strive, think, and feel with and/or in relations to others. To explain how personality develops and exerts its influence, one needs to understand the social processes driving this. And to better predict key life outcomes by personality, one needs to incorporate the embeddedness of individuals in social context.

Here, we build upon earlier reviews of personality outcomes (Soto, 2019) and development (Bleidorn et al., 2020), and the personality-relationship interplay more specifically (Back, 2021; Back, Baumert, et al., 2011; Mund et al., 2018). We provide an updated summary of the state of research, key open questions, and an agenda for the future of personality and social relationship science. We aim at a succinct presentation, but, at the same time, a broad coverage of relevant themes and approaches. Therefore, instead of giving a comprehensive overview of specific findings, we will focus on key insights and broad issues for future research illustrated by selective research examples.

What Do We Know About the Interplay of Personality and Social Relationships?

Personality and social relationships both concern somewhat stable differences in people’s experiences and behaviors, albeit on different levels. Personality is situated on the individual level and can be defined as the typical way an individual acts, thinks, wants and feels and the self-concepts that emerge in this individual. Social relationships are situated on a dyadic level and can be defined by the typical way a dyad acts, thinks, wants and feels and the relationship-concepts (e.g., relationship satisfaction) that emerge in both members of a dyad (see Back, Baumert, et al., 2011). Social relationships are shaped by the personality of both partners and their unique mixture. How much a romantic couple trusts each other, for example, is driven by how much each partner tends to trust others (an actor effect), evokes trust in others (a partner effect) and their unique tendency to trust each other that is more than the sum of their parts (a relationship effect). Social relationships, however, also provide social interaction contexts in which personality can develop. The fact that one has secured a romantic partner or has repeated interactions with friends that are characterized by aggressiveness can influence an individual’s typical experiences and behaviors across time and interaction partners, and, thus, their personality. In describing the current state of research on the interplay of personality and social relationships, we will first give an overview regarding key aspects of personality and social relationships that can be distinguished across research traditions. Afterwards, we will consider how much relationship outcomes vary across actors, partners, and specific actor-partner relations and then summarize personality effects on these variance components, that is, actor, partner, and relationship effects. Finally, we will summarize research on personality development in social relationship contexts.

Key Aspects of Personality and Social Relationships

Personality characteristics of interaction partners influence and are influenced by their social relationships. This arguably holds for all types of social relationships, although research has mostly investigated romantic (e.g., Dyrenforth et al., 2010), peer (e.g., Asendorpf & Wilpers, 1998), and family (e.g., Branje et al., 2004) relationships. Relationship functions in all of these types can be sorted according to the fundamental motives of “getting along” (attention, approval, acceptance), “getting ahead” (status, power, control of resources), and “finding meaning” (predictability, order in one’s everyday life) (e.g., Hogan & Blickle, 2018).

Relationship research has considered a large range of personality characteristics, ranging from the Big Five or HEXACO traits, temperament (particularly in childhood), attachment (particularly in parent-child and romantic relationships), and more specific characteristics such as narcissism, self-esteem, shyness, and empathy. For the sake of parsimoniousness, we will sort personality into six domains that cut across research traditions and are central in social relationships: Agentic, Communal, Sociability, Emotional Stability, Self-Control and World-View traits.

Figure 1 provides a graphical illustration of these trait domains and their potential social relevance for actors and partners in terms of the three fundamental motives of getting along, getting ahead and finding meaning. Each trait domain (e.g., Emotional Stability) is illustrated together with the typical mind-set with which social actors high in that trait domain enter the social arena (e.g., “Everything will be fine.”) and the typical mind-set that such individuals evoke in others (e.g., “won’t freak out”) via trait-typical behaviors. Please note that we have applied a male-female dyad in the figure for the ease of presentation but the logic can be applied to same-sex dyads as well. The illustration also indicates into what sorts of relationship functions these mind-sets translate over time (e.g. getting ahead: social status; getting along: lack of conflict). In the next two sections, we will examine how much these relationship outcomes vary across actors, partners and specific actor-partner relations and then summarize existing findings on how much and which personality characteristics predict these components.

Click to enlarge
ps.7505-f1
Figure 1

Socially Important Domains of Personality Differences, Respective Typical Actor and Partner Mind-Sets, and Their Link to Domains of Relationship Outcomes

Social Relationship Components: The Amount of Actor-, Partner- and Relationship Variance

An important prerequisite to understand personality effects on social relationships is to analyze the sources that contribute to differences in relationship outcomes, such as initial romantic attraction, relationship satisfaction, liking in friendships or evaluations of family members. Variance partitioning studies using the Social Relations Model (Back & Kenny, 2010) carve up the outcome measure (e.g., attraction) into three conceptually and statistically distinct components. First, to what extent is the outcome due to the attributes of the actor (e.g., some people are likers, others are dislikers)? Second, to what extent is the outcome due to the attributes of the partner (e.g., some people are likable, others are unlikable)? Third, to what extent is the outcome unique to a particular relationship (e.g., some dyads are compatible, others are incompatible)? Please note, that such variance decomposition is not restricted to basic evaluative variables such as liking or satisfaction. It can be applied to all sorts of getting along, getting ahead, and finding meaning outcomes as summarized in Figure 1. In the following, we will focus on evaluative measures that are examined often, and, thus, allow for more robust estimates.

Figure 2 depicts the current best estimates of these variance components across evaluative measures in romantic relationships (left), nonromantic peer relations (middle) and family relationship (right). Actor variance is moderate across romantic and nonromantic peer contexts, and more pronounced in family relations: That is, people differ in how they evaluate others at first encounter as well as in established relationships. Partner variance emerges, too, but it tends to be quite small in established romantic relationships: That is, people differ in how they are evaluated by others, particularly in initial romantic, peer non-romantic and family relationships. Relationship variance tends to be quite large—at least as large as actor and partner variance combined (with the exception of family relations where actor variance is as large as relationship variance)—indicating that compatibility is critically important in relationship evaluations. Interestingly, while a number of personality predictors could be identified for actor- and partner variance (blue and grey bars), relationship variance—the largest share of the variance—is, thus far, very seldomly predictable (white bars). We will have a closer look at these personality effects in the next section.

Click to enlarge
ps.7505-f2
Figure 2

Percentage of Variance in Romantic, Non-Romantic Peer and Family Relationship Evaluations due to Actor, Partner and Relationship Differences

Note. Romantic (initial) estimates derive from Asendorpf et al. (2011), Joel et al. (2017), and Payne (2011). Romantic (established) estimates derive from Eastwick and Hunt (2014, Study 3) and Eastwick et al. (2017, Study 3). Nonromantic peer estimates derive from Kenny (2019, Table 5.2). Family estimates derive from Eichelsheim et al. (2009, Affectivity results). All estimates were normed so that actor + partner + relationship = 100%, as in Kenny (2019).

Personality Effects in Social Relationships

In the following, we give a brief overview on the findings about effects of personality on social relationships that we regard as relatively robust. We will consider personality effects on all three social relationship components.

Actor Effects: Predicting Differences in how People Perceive Their Social Relationships

Emotional Stability, Communion, Sociability, and Self-Control have the most consistent positive effects on overall evaluations and expectations regarding one’s potential and existing relationships (e.g., Anglim et al., 2020). These links are particularly strong for getting along outcomes, such as self-reported satisfaction in romantic relationships (Dyrenforth et al., 2010), friendship, and peer relationships (Asendorpf & Wilpers, 1998) as well as self-reported positive family relations (e.g., Branje et al., 2004) and adaptive parenting (e.g., McCabe, 2014). Communion, open World Views, and Self-Control were also found to predict early positive evaluations across potential relationship partners (Thielmann et al., 2020). Agency relates to self-reported getting ahead outcomes such as self-perceived peer status and dating variety.

Partner Effects: Predicting Differences in how People Are Perceived by Their Social Relationship Partners

Emotional Stability is the prime predictor of partner-perceived getting along outcomes in close romantic relationships. Indeed, low Emotional Stability can be seen as “the relationship killer” because of its consistent ability to reduce not only one’s own but also partners’ relationship satisfaction (Dyrenforth et al., 2010). Communion and Self-Control are additional predictors of partner relationship satisfaction. Partner effects also play a role in family relationships: Children’s degree of Communion, Emotional Stability and Self-Control can, for example, contribute to parental warmth and stress (Ayoub et al., 2019).

Across age groups and acquaintance levels, Agency is the best predictor of social status achievement, which is necessarily defined by one’s social partners (e.g., receiving social resources, being selected as a leader; Grosz et al., 2020). These getting ahead outcomes are additionally predicted by (low) Emotional Stability, particularly for men (Anderson et al., 2001). Children and adolescents with low levels of Emotional Stability are less popular and more rejected and victimized than their peers (Salmivalli & Peets, 2018). Children with higher Self-Control are less victimized by their peers (Robson et al., 2020). Sociability−a blend of Agency and Communion−is a robust predictor of getting along with peers across age groups, both at early acquaintance (Back, Schmukle, et al., 2011) and in long-term relationships (Cillessen et al., 2011).

The Personality of the Relationship: Predicting Differences in how Specific Partners Uniquely Perceive Each Other in Their Relationship

Relationship effects pertain to effects of some sort of personality combinations of actors and partners that predict relationship outcomes above and beyond what can be predicted by actor and partner differences alone. Although such personality matching or mismatching effects are among the most typically assumed both in laypersons and researchers, empirical evidence is limited. Most research on relationship effects focuses on (dis-)similarity in personality, although similarity is only one of many possible relations between the personality characteristics of both partners (picking just one personality variable and the same one for both partners). Personality similarity has negligible and mostly inconsistent effects on initial attraction to romantic partners (e.g., Luo & Zhang, 2009) as well as romantic relationship satisfaction (e.g., Dyrenforth et al., 2010). Furthermore, the machine-learning studies of Joel and colleagues (2017, 2020) found that neither initial attraction nor established relationship satisfaction was predictable by any actor-partner combination of self-reported personality characteristics. These results indicate that, in the domain of romantic relationships, relationship variance, the biggest share of variance, must come from “something else.” For peers there is some evidence for similarity effects in the domains of Sociability and World views. For example, similarity in students’ extraversion (van Zalk et al., 2020) and adolescents’ music preferences (Selfhout, Branje, et al., 2009) predicted a higher likelihood of becoming friends and belonging to the same subgroups. It is important to note that while actual similarity is seldom found to have robust effects on relationship outcomes, perceived similarity clearly has such effects (e.g., Montoya et al., 2008).

Few studies examined more specific actor-partner personality combinations beyond simple (dis-)similarity effects. Cuperman and Ickes (2009) investigated getting acquainted interactions and showed that the effects of personality similarity may depend on the trait level on which interaction partners are similar or differ from each other. Similarity was, for example, specifically detrimental to social evaluations when two disagreeable people met. Research on romantic partners in the transition to parenthood (Marshall et al., 2015) indicated that those low in Emotional Stability react more distressed to behaviors shown by low communal partners, which might stress out the latter more and lead to more disagreeable behaviors, provoking a downward spiral that might be uniquely strong for low Emotional Stability-low Communion pairings. Table 1 provides a summary of personality effects on social relationships.

Table 1

Personality Effects on Social Relationships

Level of personality effects Key findings
Actor and partner effects • Emotional Stability, Communion, and Self-Control are related to getting along with others (e.g., more relationship satisfaction, less conflict), particularly in close relationships.
• These effects tend to be stronger from the actor perspective, partly due to the fact that actors report on both personality and relationship outcomes; but they also hold from the partner perspective.
• Consistent across age groups, Agency and Sociability are robust predictors of getting ahead and being liked among peers.
Relationship effects • Few robust results have been revealed to date.
• Most studies focused on (dis-)similarity effects and observed very small effects at best.
• If personality similarity effects were found, then mostly for Sociability and World View traits regarding their effects on getting along and finding meaning with peers in young adulthood.
• More specific personality combinations that have been found to predict unique relationship outcomes await replication.

Personality Development in Social Relationship Contexts

There is wide agreement that relationship experiences do have an effect on personality development across the life span, particularly in certain life stages such as young adulthood (Wrzus & Neyer, 2016). A first potential social source of personality development are life transitions that come along with new social tasks and roles (Bleidorn et al., 2020). According to the social investment principle, investment into new social roles that people face (e.g., as a lover, parent, co-worker) leads them to adopt behaviors and mind-sets that fit these roles, thereby changing the typical ways they act, think, want, and feel (i.e., their personality). On the one hand, such changes can affect a whole population similarly since many roles are age-graded, providing a potential explanation for mean-level changes in traits. Empirical evidence regarding main effects of social life events on personality change are, however, mixed. The most conclusive evidence concerns increases in Emotional Stability, Sociability, and Agency during the transition to the first romantic relationship, and increases in Self-Control, Communion, and Emotional Stability during the transition from school to college or work. On the other hand, role-driven changes can be a catalyst for individual differences in change since people differ in whether, when, and how they adopt certain roles and experience associated social events, a research domain that still needs to be better explored.

A second source are parenting effects on children. Indeed, empirical evidence exists for such effects, including prospective associations between dominant parenting and lower Communion (Kochanska & Kim, 2020). Importantly, associations between parenting and child behavior can not only be due to parents’ effects on their kids (e.g., parents’ cold parenting style causing low Self-Control in kids) but also the other way round (i.e., children’s low levels of Self-Control causing a cold parenting style; e.g., Kochanska & Kim, 2020).

A third source are dyadic relationships. According to identity negotiation theory (Swann & Bosson, 2008), social interactions with others provide individuals with critical knowledge about themselves, especially if these interactions repeat over time. Social interaction partners strive for congruence in self-views, yet partners’ expectations may differ from actor’s self-views. This initiates a process of identity negotiation, whereby actors and partners continuously affect each other’s perceptions. One recent study among university freshmen found that friends’ extraversion levels influenced each other so that they became more similar over time (van Zalk et al., 2020).

A fourth source are peer networks in which these dyadic relations are embedded. Following group socialization theory (Harris, 1995) individuals both contribute to existing behavioral and attitudinal group norms and adjust their own mind-set and behavior to these norms. A range of studies, particularly in childhood, adolescence and early adulthood, shows that entering and belonging to certain peer groups indeed affects behavioral regularities that are closely connected to the personality domains of World views (e.g., attitudes and prejudice) and low Communion (e.g. antisocial behavioral style; e.g., Wrzus & Neyer, 2016). For example, stronger and more persistent identifications with nonconventional peer crowds were generally associated with more problematic behaviors throughout adolescence (Doornwaard et al., 2012).

In sum, across research traditions, theories emphasize the role of social relationships for personality development. At the moment, the empirical evidence for effects of social relationships on personality development is, however, mixed, and scattered across research fields. Also, relationship effects on personality development tend to be smaller than effects of personality on social relationships. A better understanding of what drives social relationship effects on personality (at different ages) is one of the key open questions.

Key Open Questions: Processes Underlying the Personality-Social Relationship Interplay

As summarized above, research on the interplay of personality and social relationships has provided robust insights into personality effects on social relationships across a wide range of relationship types as well as strong conceptual and initial empirical evidence for relationship effects on personality development. The next challenge for both research domains is to understand how exactly—that is, by means of which processes—one can explain these effects as well as when and for whom they occur.

An Integrative Framework of Personality-Relationship Processes

Figure 3 shows an integrative framework of processes that jointly underly the interplay of personality and social relationships. The proposed model builds on and combines previous models that describe social interaction processes underlying the personality-social relationship interplay (e.g., Back, 2021; Back, Baumert, et al., 2011), and models that describe motivational, interactional, and evaluative processes underlying personality development (Geukes et al., 2018; Wrzus & Roberts, 2017). It also more explicitly includes biological processes as well as the role of social context and situational affordances. Following the PERSOC framework (Back, Baumert, et al., 2011; also see Back, 2021), individual dispositions (i.e., stable mental representations of oneself as well as biological structures that predispose to certain ways of thinking, feeling, and wanting) and relationship dispositions (i.e., stable mental representations of specific others) are thought to influence each other over time via social interaction units that are made up of behaviors and experiential states of all interaction partners. Experiential states such as situation-, other- and meta-perceptions, social cognition and affect, and biological states such as hormonal reactions of interaction partners influence each other via observable interaction behaviors, that is, by means of behavioral expression and interpersonal perception processes. The framework also includes motivational processes that take place before people enter a social interaction, such as social expectations, preferences, goals and strategies, and processes that take place after people left an interaction, such as evaluations of interaction outcomes and narration about oneself and others. It also includes the influence of social context on nested macro-((sub)culture, ingroup-outgroup-composition, social network), meso- (social role, relationship type, relationship phase), and micro-levels (social events, social situations). These social contexts imply downstream situational affordances in circumscribed social interaction units, that is contexts “for the expression of motives, goals, values, and preferences” (Reis, 2008, p. 316). Finally, it illustrates that personality and relationship characteristics do not only influence each other by means of dyadic social exchanges but also more indirectly via relations to and effects on other partners (see Partner 2 in Figure 3; Hinde, 1997). Table 2 summarizes the range of relevant processes that are typically investigated in separate research streams (see Supplementary Materials for a version with exemplary references). We think that a joint consideration of these necessarily related processes is needed. The integrative framework provides such a joint consideration and might help to advance both theoretical development and empirical insights. First, it can work as a guideline for a closer investigation of a set of key open, process-related questions that we summarize in the following. These questions are currently unresolved and our framework helps to show how they might be tackled. In doing so, it aims at a detailed description of process chains instead of more metaphorical theoretical language. Second, instead of providing just a list of interesting processes, the model elaborates how different sorts of processes interlock, allowing to more firmly integrate process insights and work towards more holistic process explanations. Third, the framework can serve as a generic model against which existing theories can be evaluated, compared and refined. For example, this regards their causal logic and completeness, parsimoniousness, and specificity as well as their overlap with other existing theories. Fourth, the framework allows to develop new, domain-specific theories that focus on defined trait domains and relationship contexts, and specify relevant variables and processes respectively.

Click to enlarge
ps.7505-f3
Figure 3

An Integrative Framework of Personality-Relationship Processes

Note. One actor and two partners are depicted but the framework can be equally applied to dyadic relationships with only one partner or to group settings with more than two partners.

Table 2

Overview of Examined Process Domains and Exemplary Processes Underlying the Interplay of Personality and Social Relationships

Process domain Process and exemplary findings
Pre-actional motivational processes Individuals prefer and select into different social situations depending on their personalities.
- Narcissists select into situations in which they can satisfy their striving for social status.
- Differences in World views including social identification with different social, ethnic and religious groups affect where and with whom youths’ spent their leisure time, what they do in those social contexts, and whom they meet.
Affective and perceptual processes People differ in how they process social information they encounter in given situations.
- Individuals high in Sociability tend to perceive situations as more sociable.
- Those high in honesty-humility perceive less conflict of interests.
People differ in the strength of specific affective reactions.
- Individuals low on traits in the domain of Emotional Stability are more sensitive to cues of social rejection and conflict.
- Individuals high in Communion react more positively to affiliative cues.
People differ in how positively, accurately, and similarly they perceive their interaction partners.
- Individuals high in Communal traits tend to view others more positively and similarly.
- Those with Borderline Personality Disorder tend to view others more negatively (e.g., as less trustworthy) and less similar to themselves.
These perceptual differences have downstream social consequences.
- More accurate personality impressions predict liking over time among new classmates and greater relationship longevity among newlyweds.
- Viewing others with greater assumed similarity, and with positive bias is associated with relationship satisfaction among romantic couples and liking among new acquaintances.
- People who are more meta-accurate—know how others’ view the self—tend be better liked by new acquaintances and have more satisfied romantic partners, while those who believe that others see their personalities positively tend to better like new acquaintances and be more satisfied with their relationships.
Behavioral processes Individuals differ in their physical, nonverbal, paraverbal, and verbal behavioral expressions, which translate into different partner perceptions which can then guide the partners social decisions and behaviors.
- Sociability expresses as more sociable behavior across a wide range of behavioral indicators and social contexts.
The expression of most traits is dependent on their activation via trait-relevant social affordances.
- Communion traits are positively linked to prosocial behavior in social situations that provide possibilities for exploitation and reciprocity.
- Traits in the domain of low Communion (e.g., narcissistic rivalry) are only observable in more intimate, interactive situations.
- Individual differences in Emotional Stability express in socially stressful situations.
Biological processes Hormones are unique in that they are released into the bloodstream and can thus affect multiple systems in the body, including the brain, in an orchestrated way. They can thus put individuals into different “modes of operation”.
- Higher levels in oxytocin and lower levels of vasopressin, for example, were associated with a stronger motivation to cooperate.
Individuals can differ regarding these hormonal modes due to genetic differences, but also because of the situations they select and the social cues they face.
- Social status challenges were related to an increase in testosterone levels among men which increased their agentic and decreased their communal personality states.
- Social threat was related to increases in cortisol levels.
Post-actional evaluative and narrative processes Individuals differ in their general evaluation of relationship experiences and in how they integrate them into social life stories.
- In line with sociometer theory, self-esteem was bidirectionally related to meta-perceived social valuation, that is, the overarching evaluation to be liked and socially included.
- Attachment avoidance was related to a less positive affective tone in one’s relationship stories.

Understanding Actor and Partner Effects of Personality on Social Relationships

Analyzing domains of processes jointly will help to get a better understanding of why and which personality aspects affect the actor’s and partner’s relationship outcomes. Importantly, this will have to consider actor and partner effects simultaneously. Also one needs to include the whole range of relevant process domains and, thus, assess not only people’s experiences but also their actual interaction behaviors, and the characteristics of the situation they are in. Figure 4a summarizes a simplified version of the integrative framework depicted in Figure 3 for how actor’s personality translates into actor and partner evaluated relationship outcomes considering the successive flow of a subset of relevant processes. Please note that we only included arrows representing processes that are needed to explain personality effects on actor and partner relationship outcomes and did not include reciprocal feedback effects on personality hat will likely emerge over time (see Figure 3, and section on “Understanding Social Relationship Effects on Personality Development” below).

Click to enlarge
ps.7505-f4a
Figure 4a

Processes Underlying Actor, Partner and Relationship Effects of Personality in Social Interactions: Actor and Partner Effects

Note. Each numbered path represents one kind of process. 1: motivational actualization, 2: behavioral expression, 3: interpersonal perception, 4: relationship evaluation, 5-8: moderation of paths 1–4 by situational affordances, 9: situation selection, 10: situation perception, 11: motivated partner perception, 12: motivated partner evaluation, 13: behavioral feedback, 14: situation modification. Please note that in dyadic relationships all paths are mirrored for the interaction partner, resulting in even more pathways. Additional arrows from personality dispositions to situational affordances and social perceptions and evaluations are not depicted. Also, we did not include arrows for reciprocal feedback effects by which personality dispositions of actors and partners are affected and affect each other (see section on “Understanding Social Relationship Effects on Personality Development” below).

Click to enlarge
ps.7505-f4b
Figure 4b

Processes Underlying Actor, Partner and Relationship Effects of Personality in Social Interactions: Relationship Effects

Note. Each numbered blue path represents one kind of process. a: unique perception of actor behavior by partner driven by partner motivation, b: unique evaluation of actor evoked social perception driven by partner motivation, c: unique partner behavior resulting from unique partner perception, d: unique social interaction situation created by unique partner behavior, e: unique actor behavior driven by unique social interaction situation. Some of the additional pathways already described in Figure 4a on actor and partner effects are included as black arrows. Additional arrows from personality dispositions to situational affordances and social perceptions and evaluations are not depicted. Also, we did not include arrows for reciprocal feedback effects by which personality dispositions of actors and partners are affected and affect each other (see section on “Understanding Social Relationship Effects on Personality Development” below).

Partner effects of personality on social relationships (e.g., those high in Agency receive more social status) can emerge when dispositional differences actualize as what people want and how much they want it (Path 1 in Figure 4a: motivational actualization; e.g., strive for status), which is then expressed in overt behavior (Path 2: behavioral expression; e.g., self-assured and dominant behavior). This behavior by the actor then leads to how actors are perceived by others (Path 3: interpersonal perception; e.g., perceived as assertive) and these perceptions then influence how partners evaluate actors and their (potential) relationship towards the actor (Path 4: relationship evaluation processes; e.g., assigning a leadership role) and how partners react towards the actor (Path 13: behavioral feedback). Situational affordances of a given relationship and situation context (e.g., providing cues for competition) can moderate how strongly trait differences are actualized (Path 5), how much they can be expressed (Path 6), how specific behaviors are interpreted (Path 7), and how partner perceptions are evaluated (Path 8). Moreover, personality can influence how much individuals enter situations with specific affordances (Path 9: situation selection; e.g., those high in Agency actively select into competition situations) and how objective situation features are perceived (Path 10: situation perception; e.g. those high in Agency perceiving competition even in relatively neutral situations), thereby strengthening the activation of trait-related motivations.

Actor effects of personality on social relationships tend to be stronger than partner effects and this is partly a consequence of the fact that they can emerge more directly via mere intrapersonal processes: Personality (e.g., low Emotional Stability) relates to certain social motivations (Path 1; e.g., avoidance of and hypersensitivity to rejection) that are intrinsically connected to default perceptions of one’s interaction partners (Path 11: motivated partner perception; e.g., chronic perception of one’s partner as rejecting) and default relationship evaluations (Path 12: motivated partner evaluation e.g., as lacking intimacy). In addition, actor effects can also emerge via partner effects (see above): The impressions and evaluations an actor evoked (e.g., those low in Emotional Stability being seen as instable and difficult), lead to certain partner motivations and related behaviors (Path 13; behavioral feedback; e.g. criticizing) that then change the interaction situation (Path 14; situation modification; e.g., from neutral to conflict discussion) and work as affordances for the actor’s own partner perceptions and relationship evaluations (Hughes et al., 2021). Thus, driven by their personality-related motivations and via respective behavioral habits, actors can create affordances that activate their personality-dependent partner perceptions. Again, the dispositional selection and perception of affordances can strengthen the above described effects.

Understanding Effects of Personality Relations and the Relationship Component

Relationship variance is the biggest component of social relationship outcomes both in early relationship stages and in established relationships. Moreover, relational perception phenomena such as the unique (meta)positivity, (meta)accuracy, and assumed similarity with which individuals perceive each other predict more distal relationship outcomes. However, we are not yet able to predict where these unique relational perceptions come from: We don’t know who will be a good match for each other based on knowledge on individuals’ personality. Two key problems in the search for relationship effects are that previous research almost exclusively focused on (1) very distal characteristics (i.e. each individual’s self-reported traits) and (2) only one very specific kind of combination of two individuals’ personalities (i.e., their similarity). When considering the actual processes that produce relationship effects, it becomes clear that more proximate, social interaction characteristics need to be investigated (e.g., Hughes et al., 2021; van Zalk et al., 2020) and variable combinations other than (dis-)similarities are needed (Back, 2021).

Figure 4b depicts a generic process model of relationship effects (also see Back, 2021; Back, Schmukle, et al., 2011). We focus on processes needed to explain the emergence of relationship effects and did not include feedback processes by which the personality of actors and partners change and influence each other (again see section on “Understanding Social Relationship Effects on Personality Development” below). Following this model, to understand who matches with whom, one needs to understand the unique ways in which individuals perceive certain expressions of their social partner. In order for relationship effects to emerge, expressions of one partner’s (A) typical behavior (or appearance) need to be perceived in unique ways by the other partner (B) according to this partner’s motivational mind-set (Path 1 in Figure 4b; e.g., nervous behaviors are perceived as weak, particularly by those with a low motivation for intimacy) and/or the perceptions one partner (A) typically evokes need to be evaluated in unique ways by the other partner (B) according to this partner’s motivational mind-set (Path 2; e.g., weakness perceptions are evaluated as annoying, particularly by those with a low motivation for intimacy). Moreover, this relationship effect can be strengthened if the resulting unique evaluation of B activates unique behaviors towards A (Path 3) which then affects the social interaction situation (Path 4) and, thus, functions as a situational affordance for further expressions of A’s behavior (Path 5). If such processes hold for many dyads of a population, a relationship effect should emerge (e.g., the relation between nervousness behavior and intimacy motivation should predict a perception and thus a relational evaluation)1. The matching, thus, regards a behavioral state of one partner and an experiential state of the other partner. Following this logic, similarity effects between self-reported traits can only evolve under very specific circumstances: The trait in question needs to predict both a certain behavioral state and influence how this same state is perceived. Researchers searching for relationship effects are well-advised to more directly capture the underlying behavioral and experiential state characteristics as well as those trait characteristics that should predict these states. Such a more fine-grained approach can be performed both in a top-down and a bottom-up fashion. Regarding the top-down approach, existing personality and relationship theories can be carefully applied to define specific behavior-motivation relations that should evoke relationship effects and test them in a confirmatory fashion. Following attachment theory, one might, for example, predict that individuals high in attachment anxiety will react especially distressed when partners engage in a style of conflict that is cold and unresponsive (Overall et al., 2022). Similarly, following social status accounts to narcissism, narcissistic individuals can be expected to react particularly aggressively to those who act dominant and, thus, are perceived as threats to social status (Grapsas et al., 2020; Mota et al., 2022). Given the myriad of possible relations between characteristics of two individuals, research might, however, additionally search for predictive relations in an exploratory fashion. Thus, regarding the bottom-up approach one might feed machine learning algorithms with a large range of experiential and behavioral states of all involved social partners to check whether and which combinations provide predictive performance. Importantly such a theory-free approach requires careful cross-validation and conceptually sensitive interpretation (Stachl et al., 2020).

Interestingly, there is some evidence that actual similarity in core personality traits may enhance perceptions of similarity, but that the effect is lagged, and needs time to unfold: For instance, among just-acquainted freshmen, actual and peer-rated similarity in personality traits longitudinally predicted more perceived similarity in these traits, which, in turn, predicted a higher likelihood of friendship formation (Selfhout, Denissen, et al., 2009). Such dynamic, longitudinal perspectives may shed more light on the processes that underlie emerging relationship effects.

Research on relationship effects might also include a broader set of variables and approaches. First, at least during acquaintanceship and conditions of uncertainty, personality reputations (i.e., what others think of a potential relationship partner) rather than self-perceptions may guide relationship choices (Selfhout, Denissen, et al., 2009). Second, one might apply narrative approaches to capture how couples make meaning out of the way that the story of their relationship fits into the story of their life (Bühler & Dunlop, 2019; Dunlop et al., 2021). Third, researchers who study established relationships should consider taking inspiration from the paradigms of person-perception scholars and incorporate additional targets into their designs (e.g., assess the focal participant’s impression of their romantic partner as well as other targets in the participant’s “field of eligibles”). By incorporating control targets in this way, researchers will be able to identify relationship effects that derive from idioms, rituals, microculture, and standards that are bound to one particular relationship in a person’s life (Eastwick, Finkel, & Joel, 2022).

Understanding the Joint and Interactive Role of Biological and Social Processes

Biological processes are not yet sufficiently integrated into research on personality and social relationships. A first major problem is that the actual state of knowledge is unclear: Many published studies in social endocrinology are characterized by small samples and thus likely underpowered. In the case of testosterone and cortisol, for example, large-scale replications and well-powered preregistered studies are still exceptions (e.g., Kordsmeyer & Penke, 2019; Sundin et al., 2021). Another hormonal candidate for the regulation of personality processes in social contexts, oxytocin, faces severe methodological problems regarding measurement and application (Quintana et al., 2021).

A second major problem is that biological processes are often studied in isolation. In the case of hormones, James Roney called for “theoretical frameworks” of the overall regulatory effects that a hormone has in humans. Hormones can have endogenous effects on social motives (e.g. effects of estradiol and progesterone across the female ovulatory cycle on sexual desire, mating motives, and attraction). They can also react to social stimuli (e.g. effects of the presence of social status challenges on testosterone, physical proximity of significant others in case of oxytocin, or of social threat on cortisol), which in turn can affect personality-relevant experiential and behavioral states (e.g. competitiveness vs. nurturance in the case of testosterone, stress reduction and social affiliation in the case of oxytocin, stress experience in the case of cortisol) (Roney, 2016).

Such more integrative research on biological and social processes will have to account for the fact that individuals can differ in the degree to which such processes play a role. Also, further biological processes, such as cardiac vagal flexibility, which might be an indicator of social sensitivity, reflecting one’s attunement to the social environment (Muhtadie et al., 2015) can be integrated. For any of these approaches, large preregistered replications are needed.

Understanding Between-Person Differences in Within-Person Social Variability

In the last two decades, research has integrated between-person differences in within-person state variability as an important aspect of personality that arise from cognitive-affective-motivational processes activated in specific situations (e.g., Fleeson, 2001). These differences in the variability of behavioral and experiential states can also be produced within relationship contexts (see Back, 2021, for an extended discussion): Varying social interactions and partner behaviors therein are key social triggers for varying actor experiences and behaviors. Moreover, research findings suggest that individual differences in within-person variability are related to interpersonal outcomes, above and beyond mean-level in behavior. For example, variability in behavior was related to less accurate perceptions by new acquaintances (Human et al., 2019), and lower relationship satisfaction (Sadikaj et al., 2015). Importantly, however, effects of variability might vary on the time-scale, the developmental context, and the specific behaviors in which variability is assessed.

Although between-person differences in within-person variability is one of the hot topics in personality-relationship research and in personality science more generally, this concept—how it is defined and measured as well as its determinants and consequences—is to date only loosely understood. We do not know what causes large within-person variability. Is large variability caused by factors within the actor, the partner, or by the interaction of actor and partner factors? Similarly, we do not know the processes via which within-person variability affects relationship outcomes.

A first key problem of this stream of research is that individual differences in variability are often considered a unitary concept where, in fact, they can be split up into a number of different sorts of variability differences. Most basically, one needs to differentiate between individual differences in how much people differ within similar social situations versus how much people differ across different social situations (Geukes et al., 2017). These two basic kinds of variability differences can be expected to have distinct determinants and social consequences: While higher values in the former can be seen as a kind of randomness or inner fragility, higher values in the latter can be seen as a sign of flexibility; adaptive responses to changing situational affordances (see Within and Across Context Variability model by Geukes et al., 2017 for details). Besides examining variability differences in the expressed level of a specific behavior, perception or affect states (flux), one can additionally examine variability differences in the extremity of expressed states (pulse) and in the kind of expressed interpersonal behaviors or perceptions (spin) (Moskowitz & Zuroff, 2004). Finally, one can simultaneously consider the variability versus coherence of people’s level of trait expression and of peoples expressed personality profiles (Sherman et al., 2012).

A second key problem in this research area is that variability differences are considered as to represent psychological entities that can, in itself, have causal effects on relationship outcomes. Variabilities might be better dealt with as dynamic proxy measures, by-products of underlying social processes, specifically of the degree to which people react psychologically to more or less varying social affordances. Individual differences in within-context variability, can, for example, be seen as the result of differences in the stability of if-then contingencies, while individual differences in between-context variability can be seen as the result of differences in the strength of if-then contingencies (Back, 2021).

Understanding Social Relationship Effects on Personality Development

Research on the specific social mechanisms that drive relationship effects on personality development is still in its infancy. Most of the previous research addressed relationship processes theoretically but measured relationships in a generalized way and retrospectively over several years (Wrzus & Neyer, 2016).

One kind of process that seems to relate to both short-term variations and long-term changes in personality, particularly in the domain of Agency, are effects of meta-perceptions. Repeatedly feeling socially included, accepted and/or valued by one’s peers, for example, related to increases in self-esteem (i.e., sociometer processes; e.g. Hutteman et al., 2015). Stress reactivity, or reactivity to daily hassles (including social conflicts), seems to be one process that particularly drives changes in Emotional (In)Stability (Borghuis et al., 2020; Wrzus et al., 2021). Behavioral mimicry, the tendency to adopt typical behaviors of one’s social partners, might be particularly relevant when it comes to extraversion development within peer groups (van Zalk et al., 2020).

Such behavioral processes, particularly the way people communicate with and perceive each other (or abstain from communication), are also crucial to better understand the development of World view traits, for example attitudes and prejudice. The positivity and negativity of intergroup contact (i.e., encounters with members of other groups) relates to the development of intergroup attitudes and ethnic identities (Schäfer et al., 2021). However, what actually happens during social encounters with outgroup members that are perceived as “positive” or “negative” in retrospect is currently unclear.

Social relationships can also influence personality via their effect on biological processes. For example, committed romantic relationships and parenthood are associated with reduced testosterone levels though this effect seems to be moderated by orientation towards offspring investment and pair-bonding (Edelstein et al., 2011). Presence of potential romantic partners, on the other hand, increases male testosterone (Kordsmeyer & Penke, 2019; Roney, 2016). Moreover, adverse childhood experiences can have lasting effects on the stress system (Shonkoff et al., 2012). How far and under which conditions such effects translate into lasting personality changes is unclear. More integrative research on concrete social and biological mechanisms that underlie long-term trait development is needed (also see Back, 2021; Bleidorn et al., 2020; Geukes et al., 2018; Wrzus & Roberts, 2017).

Understanding the Role of Social Context

Most research focuses on the relations of personality in one specific type of relationship. Following the working model depicted in Figure 3, a closer consideration of social context is needed. This regards the embedding of relationships in social networks, the transition across phases within relationships, and the specific situations relationships are made of and provide.

On a macro level, individuals tend to have multiple, interconnected relationships, which form larger social networks (Wrzus et al., 2013). Relationships influence and are influenced by other individuals in a social network and by how the network is structured. A good illustration of this is provided by adolescence, a developmental period when youths’ social worlds become increasingly complex. The friends of adolescents’ friends may know each other, interact, and become friends as well and such indirect friendships can contribute to personality development. In the domain of World Views, for example, indirect friendships predicted the development of intergroup attitudes (Wölfer et al., 2019). Simply put, the answer to the question “who am I?” is likely to not only depend on one’s self-perceptions and relationship with another person, but also on how this other person’s relationships with yet other people develop and the social network in which these relationships are embedded. To better understand such network effects, one needs a stronger incorporation of the role of communication processes. This includes, for example, communication among different relationship partners about an actor (aka “gossip”; Costello & Srivastava, 2021).

On a meso level, personality research currently lacks a more integrative analysis of different phases of relationship formation including relationship selection, stabilization, maintenance, and dissolution. For all sorts of relationship types, research mostly focused on either relationship initiation or relationship maintenance. Research indicates that personality-relationship associations differ between short- and long-term relationship contexts. Communal personality aspects, for example, seem to exert a stronger influence in later as compared to earlier relationship phases (e.g., see Leckelt et al., 2015 in the case of antagonistic narcissism). There is longitudinal research on the first few months of an initial acquaintanceship and on established relationships, but there is a gap in between. The key challenge is, thus, to more fully track relationships from the very beginning to the development of a long-term meaningful relationship. Even more challenging will be to truly capture relationship transitions, that is to follow people (a) before they meet until they form a relationship and (b) from one relationship to another relationship. Analyzing such transitions is important to being able to disentangle (a) what attributes people “brought with them” into the relationship vs. the attributes that changed as a consequence of that relationship, and (b) what aspects of experiences and behaviors in a relationship are due to this specific relationship, this type of relationship more generally, and this individual. Examining relationship transitions is challenging because the number of initial interactions that turn into a meaningful relationship, as well as the numbers of individuals in existing relationships that enter a new relationship are quite low in typical research designs.

On a micro-level, relationships are lived within circumscribed situations that have specific affordances for certain interaction behaviors. These affordances and how they are perceived and thereby influence individuals’ behaviors need to be more fully considered. Affordances may vary across relationship types, and across cultures. They determine how and how much personality is expressed and observable and they may also moderate how expressed individual differences are evaluated (see Figure 4Figure 4b). Different relationships may likewise provide different affordances for behavior. If someone has a very good relationship with their employer, for example, an interaction may be much less constrained and also provide an affordance for Sociability, not only Self-Control. Affordances may thus be key to explain why certain traits only unfold in certain types of relationships, but not in others—affordances may also change over the course of a relationship and across interaction partners.

Conclusion: A Call for Conceptual Integration, Methodological Expansion, and Collaborative Action

Research on personality and social relationships has exploded in the past 10–20 years. Across diverse subdisciplines, it has contributed to a much better understanding of how the interplay between who we are—our personality—and how we are with others−our relationships. Understanding this interplay is key to understanding personality itself, that is, as regularities in how people strive for and select into social situations, and act towards and perceive others in these situations. It is also important in understanding its social outcomes and further life outcomes that are influenced by relationships, as well as changes and stabilization of personality in the context of relationships. Despite extensive progress and increasingly ambitious work that has been done, research on personality and social relationships is still scattered across fields and there are many unknowns and pressing unresolved questions.

First, there is need for conceptual integration across many independent lines of research that deal with very similar topics but engage in comparatively little cross-talk. The challenge here is to enable a broad and diverse consideration of personality, social relationship, and process aspects across literatures but at the same time to foster a stronger integration. That is, as we have showcased in this article, relationship research in personality psychology needs to more strongly include social, developmental, and network perspectives. In doing so, research will have to bridge the gaps between different aspects of personality, between different relationship types and phases, as well as between different process domains. This comes along with the need for parsimoniousness: The myriad of concepts and labels need to be condensed into key concepts and processes to reduce redundancy and jingle-jangle fallacies. This is not meant in a prescriptive way that limits available or new approaches. Quite in contrast, we want to highlight that conceptual and methodological diversity is incredibly important and particularly characteristic of personality research as a hub science. This diversity does, however, not preclude a better conceptual integration and a common language. That is, while “one should certainly ‘let all flowers bloom’ […] this does not mean that one should not try to better understand the similarities and distinct features of these flowers; to come closer to a systematic understanding of their overarching structures, appearances, and developmental principles; and to talk about these flowers in systemized scientific ways (e.g. using different names for different flowers and the same names for the same flowers and having verbal systems to sort flowers according to their (dis)similarities).” (Back, 2020, p. 4).

Second, in order to sufficiently capture and analyze personality and social relationships as well as the processes underlying this interplay, there is a need for methodological expansion. Regarding both the assessment of personality and social relationship characteristics, research needs to move beyond self-reports and include other data sources such as informant-reports, implicit tests, and behavioral tasks. To capture relevant processes, one has to assess experiential, behavioral, and biological states in the moment (e.g., Geukes et al., 2019; Wrzus & Mehl, 2015). Importantly each assessment tool should match the phenomenon that is to be assessed. People’s self-concept and their overarching relationship concepts are, for example, best assessed with global decontextualized self-report questionnaires. Indirect assessment tools might be needed to capture motives or relationship representations that are less explicitly accessible. Experience-sampled self-reports might capture people’s affect, cognition, and perceptions in the moment, while direct observations are needed to capture their behavior, and experienced-sampled other-reports might be applied to capture how people come across during social interactions. It’s not just different methods capturing the same thing but different aspects that are best captured by different methods. Time-wise, one needs to capture relevant short-term (within day, daily, monthly) and long term (e.g. yearly) processes. One option is, thus, to set up large longitudinal studies that combine the repeated assessment of personality and relationship aspects (e.g., self- and informant-report surveys) with measurement bursts in which more detailed social processes are assessed (e.g., via experience sampling, social sensing, laboratory experiments, virtual reality) (e.g., Geukes et al., 2019; Wrzus et al., 2021). Importantly, to obtain robust and generalizable results, the investigation of intensive process data has to be combined with much larger sample sizes than usually done. To analyze the longitudinal interplay of personality and social relationships with such resulting complex data structures, sophisticated tools for data analysis are needed (e.g., Nestler et al., 2015).

The outlined challenges can most likely not be solved by individual research teams alone. Thus, there is, third, a need for collaborative action. Researchers across relevant (sub)disciplines should engage in coordinated theoretical, empirical, and methodological work. This might, for example, include conceptual integration efforts across different subdisciplines, coordinated intensive data collections, and mega-analyses across large existing data sets. Such collaborative action will move the field forward towards a detailed understanding of the exciting interplay of personality and social relationships.

Notes

1) Please note, that the relation can be of many kinds including interactive moderation effects, absolute, or directed discrepancy effects. The relations of interest need to be defined in order to apply appropriate statistical tests.

Funding

The authors have no funding to report.

Acknowledgments

We thank Leonie Hater for her help with the preparation of Figures 1 and 3.

Competing Interests

Isabel Thielmann and Cornelia Wrzus are members of the journal’s editorial board, Lauren Human is an associate editor of the journal, and Mitja Back is a senior consultant member to the journal.

Author Contributions

Mitja D. Back—Idea, conceptualization | Visualization (data presentation, figures, etc.) | Writing | Feedback, revisions. Susan Branje—Idea, conceptualization | Writing | Feedback, revisions. Paul W. Eastwick—Idea, conceptualization | Visualization (data presentation, figures, etc.) | Writing | Feedback, revisions. Lauren J. Human—Idea, conceptualization | Writing | Feedback, revisions. Lars Penke—Idea, conceptualization | Writing | Feedback, revisions. Gentiana Sadikaj—Idea, conceptualization | Writing | Feedback, revisions. Richard B. Slatcher—Idea, conceptualization | Writing | Feedback, revisions. Isabel Thielmann—Idea, conceptualization | Writing | Feedback, revisions. Maarten H. W. van Zalk—Idea, conceptualization | Writing | Feedback, revisions. Cornelia Wrzus—Idea, conceptualization | Writing | Feedback, revisions.

Supplementary Materials

For this article, an overview of the process domains and exemplary processes including references is available as a supplement (for access see Index of Supplementary Materials below).

Index of Supplementary Materials

  • Back, M. D., Branje, S., Eastwick, P. W., Human, L. J., Penke, L., Sadikaj, G., Slatcher, R. B., Thielmann, I., van Zalk, M. H. W., & Wrzus, C. (2022). Supplementary materials to "Personality and social relationships: What do we know and where do we go" [Table]. PsychOpen GOLD. https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.12341

References

  • Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (2001). Who attains social status? Effects of personality and physical attractiveness in social groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 116-132. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.116

  • Anglim, J., Horwood, S., Smillie, L. D., Marrero, R. J., & Wood, J. K. (2020). Predicting psychological and subjective well-being from personality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(4), 279-323. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000226

  • Asendorpf, J. B., Penke, L., & Back, M. D. (2011). From dating to mating and relating: Predictors of initial and long-term outcomes of speed-dating in a community sample. European Journal of Personality, 25(1), 16-30. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.768

  • Asendorpf, J. B., & Wilpers, S. (1998). Personality effects on social relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1531-1544. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1531

  • Ayoub, M., Briley, D. A., Grotzinger, A., Patterson, M. W., Engelhardt, L. E., Tackett, J. L., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2019). Genetic and environmental associations between child personality and parenting. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 10(6), 711-721. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550618784890

  • Back, M. D. (2020). Editorial: A brief wish list for personality research. European Journal of Personality, 34(1), 3-7. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2236

  • Back, M. D. (2021). Social interaction processes and personality. In J. Rauthmann (Ed.), The handbook of personality dynamics and processes (pp. 183–226). Elsevier.

  • Back, M. D., Baumert, A., Denissen, J. J. A., Hartung, F.-M., Penke, L., Schmukle, S. C., Schönbrodt, F. D., Schröder-Abé, M., Vollmann, M., Wagner, J., & Wrzus, C. (2011). PERSOC: A unified framework for understanding the dynamic interplay of personality and social relationships. European Journal of Personality, 25(2), 90-107. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.811

  • Back, M. D., & Kenny, D. A. (2010). The Social Relations Model: How to understand dyadic processes. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(10), 855-870. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00303.x

  • Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2011). A closer look at first sight: Social relations lens model analyses of personality and interpersonal attraction at zero acquaintance. European Journal of Personality, 25(3), 225-238. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.790

  • Bleidorn, W., Hopwood, C. J., Back, M. D., Denissen, J. J. A., Hennecke, M., Jokela, M., Kandler, C., Lucas, R. E., Luhmann, M., Orth, U., Roberts, B. W., Wagner, J., Wrzus, C., & Zimmermann, J. (2020). Longitudinal experience-wide association studies (LEWAS) – A framework for studying personality change. European Journal of Personality, 34(3), 285-300. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2247

  • Borghuis, J., Bleidorn, W., Sijtsma, K., Branje, S., Meeus, W. H. J., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2020). Longitudinal associations between trait neuroticism and negative daily experiences in adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118(2), 348-363. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000233

  • Branje, S. J. T., van Lieshout, C. F. M., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2004). Relations between Big Five personality characteristics and perceived support in adolescents’ families. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(4), 615-628. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.4.615

  • Bühler, J. L., & Dunlop, W. L. (2019). The narrative identity approach and romantic relationships. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 13(4), Article e12447. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12447

  • Cillessen, A. H. N., Schwartz, D., & Mayeux, L. L. (Eds.) (2011). Popularity in the peer system. The Guilford Press.

  • Costello, C. K., & Srivastava, S. (2021). Perceiving personality through the grapevine: A network approach to reputations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121(1), 151-167. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000362

  • Cuperman, R., & Ickes, W. (2009). Big Five predictors of behavior and perceptions in initial dyadic interactions: Personality similarity helps extraverts and introverts, but hurts “disagreeables”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(4), 667-684. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015741

  • Doornwaard, S. M., Branje, S., Meeus, W. H. J., & Ter Bogt, T. F. M. (2012). Development of adolescents’ peer crowd identification in relation to changes in problem behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 48(5), 1366-1380. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026994

  • Dunlop, W. L., Bühler, J., Maghsoodi, A., Harake, N., Wilkinson, D., & McAdams, D. P. (2021). The stories couples live by. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 38(2), 690-710. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407520969900

  • Dyrenforth, P. S., Kashy, D. A., Donnellan, M. B., & Lucas, R. E. (2010). Predicting relationship and life satisfaction from personality in nationally representative samples from three countries: The relative importance of actor, partner, and similarity effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(4), 690-702. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020385

  • Eastwick, P. W., Finkel, E. J., & Joel, S. (2022). Mate evaluation theory. Psychological Review. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000360

  • Eastwick, P. W., Harden, K. P., Shukusky, J. A., Morgan, T. A., & Joel, S. (2017). Consistency and inconsistency among romantic partners over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(6), 838-859. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000087

  • Eastwick, P. W., & Hunt, L. L. (2014). Relational mate value: Consensus and uniqueness in romantic evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(5), 728-751. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035884

  • Edelstein, R. S., Chopik, W. J., & Kean, E. L. (2011). Sociosexuality moderates the association between testosterone and relationship status in men and women. Hormones and Behavior, 60(3), 248-255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.05.007

  • Eichelsheim, V. I., Deković, M., Buist, K. L., & Cook, W. L. (2009). The social relations model in family studies: A systematic review. Journal of Marriage and Family, 71(4), 1052-1069. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2009.00652.x

  • Fleeson, W. (2001). Towards a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(6), 1011-1027. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.1011

  • Geukes, K., Breil, S. M., Hutteman, R., Nestler, S., Küfner, A. C. P., & Back, M. D. (2019). Explaining the longitudinal interplay of personality and social relationships in the laboratory and in the field: The PILS and the CONNECT study. PLoS One, 14(1), Article e0210424. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210424

  • Geukes, K., Nestler, S., Hutteman, R., Küfner, A. C. P., & Back, M. D. (2017). Trait personality and state variability: Predicting individual differences in within- and cross-context fluctuations in affect, self-evaluations, and behavior in everyday life. Journal of Research in Personality, 69, 124-138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.06.003

  • Geukes, K., van Zalk, M., & Back, M. D. (2018). Understanding personality development: An integrative state process model. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 42(1), 43-51. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025416677847

  • Grapsas, S., Brummelman, E., Back, M. D., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2020). The “why” and “how” of narcissism: A process model of narcissistic status pursuit. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(1), 150-172. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619873350

  • Grosz, M. P., Leckelt, M., & Back, M. D. (2020). Personality predictors of social status attainment. Current Opinion in Psychology, 33, 52-56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.023

  • Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child’s environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102(3), 458-489. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.458

  • Hinde, R. A. (1997). Relationships: A dialectical perspective. Psychology Press.

  • Hogan, R., & Blickle, G. (2018). Socioanalytic theory: Basic concepts, supporting evidences, and practical implications. In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality and individual differences: The science of personality and individual differences (pp. 110–129). Sage Reference. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781526451163.n5

  • Hughes, B. T., Flournoy, J. C., & Srivastava, S. (2021). Is perceived similarity more than assumed similarity? An interpersonal path to seeing similarity between self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121(1), 184-200. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000369

  • Human, L. J., Mignault, M. C., Biesanz, J. C., & Rogers, K. H. (2019). Why are well-adjusted people seen more accurately? The role of personality-behavior congruence in naturalistic social settings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(2), 465-482. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000193

  • Hutteman, R., Nestler, S., Wagner, J., Egloff, B., & Back, M. D. (2015). Wherever I may roam: Processes of self-esteem development from adolescence to emerging adulthood in the context of international student exchange. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(5), 767-783. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000015

  • Joel, S., Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2017). Is romantic desire predictable? Machine learning applied to initial romantic attraction. Psychological Science, 28(10), 1478-1489. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617714580

  • Joel, S., Eastwick, P. W., Allison, C. J., Arriaga, X. B., Baker, Z. G., Bar-Kalifa, E., Bergeron, S., Birnbaum, G., Brock, R. L., Brumbaugh, C. C., Carmichael, C. L., Chen, S., Clarke, J., Cobb, R. J., Coolsen, M. K., Davis, J., de Jong, D. C., Debrot, A., DeHaas, E. C., . . .Wolf, S. (2020). Machine learning uncovers the most robust self-report predictors of relationship quality across 43 longitudinal couples studies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117(32), 19061-19071. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1917036117

  • Kenny, D. A. (2019). Interpersonal perception: The foundation of social relationships. Guilford Publications.

  • Kochanska, G., & Kim, S. (2020). Children’s early difficulty and agreeableness in adolescence: Testing a developmental model of interplay of parent and child effects. Developmental Psychology, 56(8), 1556-1564. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001023

  • Kordsmeyer, T. L., & Penke, L. (2019). Effects of male testosterone and its interaction with cortisol on self- and observer-rated personality states in a competitive mating context. Journal of Research in Personality, 78, 76-92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.11.001

  • Leckelt, M., Küfner, A. C. P., Nestler, S., & Back, M. D. (2015). Behavioral processes underlying the decline of narcissists’ popularity over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(5), 856-871. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000057

  • Luo, S., & Zhang, G. (2009). What leads to romantic attraction: Similarity, reciprocity, security, or beauty? Evidence from a speed‐dating study. Journal of Personality, 77(4), 933-964. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00570.x

  • Marshall, E. M., Simpson, J. A., & Rholes, W. S. (2015). Personality, communication, and depressive symptoms across the transition to parenthood: A dyadic longitudinal investigation. European Journal of Personality, 29(2), 216-234. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1980

  • McCabe, J. E. (2014). Maternal personality and psychopathology as determinants of parenting behavior: A quantitative integration of two parenting literatures. Psychological Bulletin, 140(3), 722-750. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034835

  • Montoya, R. M., Horton, R. S., & Kirchner, J. (2008). Is actual similarity necessary for attraction? A meta-analysis of actual and perceived similarity. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 25(6), 889-922. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407508096700

  • Moskowitz, D. S., & Zuroff, D. C. (2004). Flux, pulse, and spin: Dynamic additions to the personality lexicon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(6), 880-893. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.6.880

  • Mota, S., Mielke, I., Kroencke, L., Geukes, K., Nestler, S., & Back, M. D. (2022). The daily dynamics of grandiose narcissism: Distribution, stability, and trait-relations of admiration and rivalry states and state contingencies. European Journal of Personality. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/08902070221081322

  • Mõttus, R., Wood, D., Condon, D. M., Back, M. D., Baumert, A., Costantini, G., Epskamp, S., Greiff, S., Johnson, W., Lukaszewski, A., Murray, A., Revelle, W., Wright, A., Yarkoni, T., Ziegler, M., & Zimmermann, J. (2020). Descriptive, predictive and explanatory personality research: Different goals, different approaches, but a shared need to move beyond the Big Few traits. European Journal of Personality, 34(6), 1175-1201. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/hvk5p

  • Mund, M., Jeronimus, B. F., & Neyer, F. J. (2018). Personality and social relationships: As thick as thieves. In C. Johansen (Ed.), Your personality makes you ill: Scientific proof or wishful thinking? (pp. 153–183). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/gm8qv

  • Muhtadie, L., Koslov, K., Akinola, M., & Mendes, W. B. (2015). Vagal flexibility: A physiological predictor of social sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 106-120. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000016

  • Nestler, S., Grimm, K. J., & Schönbrodt, F. D. (2015). The social consequences and mechanisms of personality: How to analyse longitudinal data from individual, dyadic, round-robin, and network designs. European Journal of Personality, 29(2), 272-295. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1997

  • Overall, N. C., Pietromonaco, P. R., & Simpson, J. A. (2022). Buffering and spillover of adult attachment insecurity in couple and family relationships. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1, 101-111. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-021-00011-1

  • Payne, K. T. (2011). Does psychopathy predict desirability in speed dating situations? A social relations analysis [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Southern Mississippi.

  • Quintana, D. S., Lischke, A., Grace, S., Scheele, D., Ma, Y., & Becker, B. (2021). Advances in the field of intranasal oxytocin research: Lessons learned and future directions for clinical research. Molecular Psychiatry, 26(1), 80-91. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-020-00864-7

  • Reis, H. T. (2008). Reinvigorating the concept of situation in social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12(4), 311-329. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868308321721

  • Robson, D. A., Allen, M. S., & Howard, S. J. (2020). Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 146(4), 324-354. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000227

  • Roney, J. R. (2016). Theoretical frameworks for human behavioral endocrinology. Hormones and Behavior, 84, 97-110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.06.004

  • Sadikaj, G., Rappaport, L. M., Moskowitz, D. S., Zuroff, D. C., Koestner, R., & Powers, T. (2015). Consequences of interpersonal spin on couple-relevant goal progress and relationship satisfaction in romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(4), 722-737. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000043

  • Salmivalli, C., & Peets, K. (2018). Bullying and vicitmization. In W. M. Bukowski, B. Laursen, & K. H. Rubin (Eds), Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups (2nd ed., pp. 159–176). The Guilford Press.

  • Schäfer, S. J., Kauff, M., Prati, F., Kros, M., Lang, T., & Christ, O. (2021). Does negative contact undermine attempts to improve intergroup relations? Deepening the understanding of negative contact and its consequences for intergroup contact research and interventions. The Journal of Social Issues, 77(1), 197-216. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12422

  • Selfhout, M. H. W., Denissen, J., Branje, S., & Meeus, W. (2009). In the eye of the beholder: Perceived, actual, and peer-rated similarity in personality, communication, and friendship intensity during the acquaintanceship process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(6), 1152-1165. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014468

  • Selfhout, M. H. W., Branje, S. J., ter Bogt, T. F., & Meeus, W. H. (2009). The role of music preferences in early adolescents’ friendship formation and stability. Journal of Adolescence, 32(1), 95-107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.11.004

  • Sherman, R. A., Nave, C. S., & Funder, D. C. (2012). Properties of persons and situations related to overall and distinctive personality-behavior congruence. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(1), 87-101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2011.12.006

  • Shonkoff, J. P., Garner, A. S., Siegel, B. S., Dobbins, M. I., Earls, M. F., Garner, A. S., McGuinn, L., Pascoe, J., Wood, D. L., & Committee on psychosocial aspects of child and family health, Committee on early childhood, adoption, and dependent care, Section on developmental and behavioral pediatrics. (2012). The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics, 129(1), e232-e246. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-2663

  • Soto, C. J. (2019). How replicable are links between personality traits and consequential life outcomes? The life outcomes of personality replication project. Psychological Science, 30(5), 711-727. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619831612

  • Stachl, C., Pargent, F., Hilbert, S., Harari, G. M., Schoedel, R., Vaid, S., Gosling, S. D., & Bühner, M. (2020). Personality research and assessment in the era of machine learning. European Journal of Personality, 34(5), 613-631. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2257

  • Sundin, Z. W., Chopik, W. J., Welker, K. M., Ascigil, E., Brandes, C. M., Chin, K. A., Ketay, S., Knight, E. L., Kordsmeyer, T. L., McLarney-Vesotski, A. R., Prasad, S., Reese, Z. A., Roy, A. R. K., Sim, L., Stern, J., Carré, J. M., Edelstein, R. S., Mehta, P. H., Penke, L., . . .Tackett, J. L. (2021). Estimating the associations between big five personality traits, testosterone, and cortisol. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 7, 307-340. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-020-00159-9

  • Swann, W. B., Jr., & Bosson, J. K. (2008). Identity negotiation: A theory of self and social interaction. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 448–471). The Guilford Press.

  • Thielmann, I., Hilbig, B. E., & Zettler, I. (2020). Seeing me, seeing you: Testing competing accounts of assumed similarity in personality judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118(1), 172-198. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000222

  • van Zalk, M. H. W., Nestler, S., Geukes, K., Hutteman, R., & Back, M. D. (2020). The codevelopment of extraversion and friendships: Bonding and behavioral interaction mechanisms in friendship networks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118(6), 1269-1290. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000253

  • Wölfer, R., Christ, O., Schmid, K., Tausch, N., Buchallik, F. M., Vertovec, S., & Hewstone, M. (2019). Indirect contact predicts direct contact: Longitudinal evidence and the mediating role of intergroup anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(2), 277-295. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000146

  • Wrzus, C., Hänel, M., Wagner, J., & Neyer, F. J. (2013). Social network change and life events across the lifespan: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 53-80. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028601

  • Wrzus, C., Luong, G., Wagner, G. G., & Riediger, M. (2021). Longitudinal coupling of momentary stress reactivity and trait neuroticism: Specificity of states, traits, and age period. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121(3), 691-706. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000308

  • Wrzus, C., & Mehl, M. (2015). Lab and/or field? Measuring personality processes and their social consequences. European Journal of Personality, 29(2), 250-271. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1986

  • Wrzus, C., & Neyer, F. J. (2016). Co-development of personality and friendships across the lifespan: An empirical review on selection and socialization. European Psychologist, 21(4), 254-273. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000277

  • Wrzus, C., & Roberts, B. W. (2017). Processes of personality development in adulthood: The TESSERA framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(3), 253-277. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868316652279