Anxiety is the leading mental health problem reported at universities today (LeViness et al., 2017). Statistics anxiety is a domain-specific facet of anxiety pertaining to performance-based fears of statistics, low perceived self-worth in the statistics domain, and negative attitudes towards statistical education (Onwuegbuzie & Wilson, 2003). Statistics anxiety negatively influences academic performance and personal well-being (Beurze et al., 2013; Tremblay et al., 2000), sometimes inhibiting students’ ability to obtain their degree. Math anxiety (Jameson & Fusco, 2014) consists of apprehension towards events related to math, such as solving mathematical problems or being evaluated for mathematic ability (Hopko et al., 2003).
Established risk factors for statistics anxiety include epistemological anxiety, mathematical self-concept, and self-esteem (Onwuegbuzie & Wilson, 2003). Problems with mathematical self-concept and self-worth are risk factors for math anxiety (Jameson & Fusco, 2014). While statistics and math anxieties are strongly correlated, they emerge as separate constructs in factor analysis, and differ insomuch that they are content-related anxieties experienced in different contexts with varying antecedents (Paechter et al., 2017). With the advent of computing, modern statistics education has moved away from a heavy focus on arithmetic and probability theory to greater emphasis on statistical thinking, conceptual understanding, interpretation, and technology use (GAISE College Report ASA Revision Committee, 2016). Onwuegbuzie & Wilson (2003) suggest statistics education (and the accompanying anxiety) often bears more in common with learning a new language than with math education. In this study, we examined protective and risk factors for statistics and math anxiety.
Protective and Risk Factors
Self-efficacy refers to a person’s individual beliefs in their capability to succeed in tasks. In Bandura’s (1988) social-cognitive theory, people high in self-efficacy believe they can exercise control over environmental threats. Bandura argues the perception of control over such threats eliminates the fear—the feeling of helplessness in the face of threats is the primary source of anxiety. Self-efficacy influences students’ academic performance and their ability to learn (Barrows et al., 2013). Self-efficacy is also negatively correlated with math anxiety (Akin & Kurbanoglu, 2011; Jameson & Fusco, 2014). Further, self-efficacy has been linked to anxious arousal—individuals who are not confident in their coping abilities might experience anxious arousal towards challenges (Bandura, 1988). Self-efficacy is negatively associated with statistics anxiety in graduate students (Perepiczka et al., 2011; Onwuegbuzie, 2000).
Anxiety sensitivity is a personality trait that measures fear of anxiety and sensations related to anxiety arousal (Taylor et al., 2007). Thus, anxiety symptoms themselves become a self-perpetuating source of fear for people high in this trait. We suspect anxiety sensitivity plays a role in domain-specific statistics anxiety in a similar fashion as generalized clinical anxiety. Anxiety sensitivity is positively associated with self-handicapping tendencies, posing negative implications for academic performance (Kalyon et al., 2016). To our knowledge, nobody has examined the link between anxiety sensitivity and math/statistics anxiety.
We focus on two sub-dimensions of perfectionism from Frost et al. (1990): Concern over mistakes (rigid black-and white thinking, negative reactions to failure) and doubts about actions (perseverating on tasks accompanied by a sensation that work is never quite “right”). Perfectionistic students might be more anxious in statistics classrooms because there are multiple solutions to many statistical problems—the antithesis of the perfectionist’s desire to find the single “right” answer to a problem (Onwuegbuzie & Wilson, 2003). For perfectionists, the emotional impact of each mistake can be large, because they feel like the respect of other people depends upon perfect performance (Frost et al., 1990). Prior research found that statistics anxiety is modestly correlated with related perfectionism facets: socially prescribed perfectionism (Onwuegbuzie & Daley, 1999; Walsh & Ugumba-Agwunobi, 2002) and perfectionistic discrepancies (Comerchero & Fortugno, 2013). Concern over mistakes and doubts about actions are negatively correlated with math performance (Soleymani & Rekabdar, 2010), though evidence linking perfectionism and math anxiety remains limited.
A meta-analysis of 7 studies (N = 1692) showed an average correlation of 0.35 between a measure of math anxiety and a measure of general anxiety (Hembree, 1990). Walsh and Ugumba-Agwunobi (2002) found that three subdimensions of the STARS (interpretation anxiety, test and class anxiety, fear of asking for help) are correlated with trait anxiety. Bandura (1988) argues that feeling in control can eliminate fear, as feeling helpless is the primary source of anxiety. Both anxiety sensitivity and facets of perfectionism are positively related to general anxiety (Hovenkamp-Hermelink et al., 2019; Smith et al., 2018).
Multidimensionality of Statistics Anxiety
Statistics anxiety is a multifaceted construct that can be split into “anxiety” and “attitudes” towards statistics (Onwuegbuzie & Wilson, 2003). Three of Cruise et al.’s (1985) six factors tap this “anxiety” construct: test and class anxiety, interpretation anxiety, and fear of asking for help. In contrast, worth of statistics, fear of statistics teachers, and computational self-concept occupy the “attitudes” conceptual component of statistics anxiety. That is, attitudes about oneself or statistics as a discipline, rather than the emotional state of anxiety.
There is little research that specifically discusses self-efficacy, anxiety sensitivity, concern over mistakes and/or doubts about actions and their associations with Cruise et al.’s (1985) six statistics anxiety factors. Nonetheless, we might expect that anxiety sensitivity and perfectionism would be more strongly related to the three “anxiety” factors, given much research showing a positive correlation with these traits and generalized anxiety (Taylor et al., 2007; Walsh & Ugumba-Agwunobi, 2002).
Important Covariates
Students in arts programs experience higher levels of math anxiety relative to students in science programs (O’Leary et al., 2017). This anxiety might stem from fewer high school math requirements in arts programs, leading to anxiety from a lack of math training. Therefore, we will control for university program (arts vs. science vs. other).
Graduate students are more than six times as likely to experience mental health problems—including anxiety—than the general population (Evans et al., 2018). Given that statistics anxiety impacts students across education levels (Onwuegbuzie, 2000; Onwuegbuzie & Wilson, 2003) and may contribute to higher anxiety among graduate students, post-secondary students at undergraduate and graduate levels are our population of interest. These two samples—undergraduate vs. graduate—present a second covariate for our study.
Finally, women tend to score higher than men on traits corresponding with negative emotionality, such as neuroticism (Schmitt et al., 2008), and are almost twice as likely to be affected by anxiety disorders than men regardless of culture (Remes et al., 2016). Girls’ performance on math is more influenced by their math anxiety than boys; moreover, girls’ math performance is associated with their math anxiety, even after controlling for general test anxiety (Devine et al., 2012). Thus, gender is a third covariate.
Rationale
Statistics/math anxiety is an important topic to study because it has known negative effects on performance and engagement in statistics classrooms (Onwuegbuzie & Wilson, 2003). Moreover, anxiety is a naturally aversive state that most instructors would hope to minimize in their classroom to promote well-being in their students. Studying how personality is related to statistics anxiety may help us understand which students are at higher risk for statistics anxiety, to better inform future applied intervention research. There are numerous gaps in knowledge we attempted to fill. Undergraduate students are over-represented (Onwuegbuzie & Wilson, 2003), so we over-sampled graduate students. While studies have identified individual difference factors for math anxiety (e.g., nonverbal number skills; Ahmed et al., 2012), little research has tested the relationship between individual difference factors and statistics anxiety. Studies (e.g., Comerchero & Fortugno, 2013) tend to have small sample sizes (< 200), resulting in relatively imprecise estimates. That is, they could detect non-zero relationships, but precision of effect sizes tended to be low. To our knowledge, nobody has examined the link between anxiety sensitivity and math/statistics anxiety, despite well-known theory suggesting that anxiety sensitivity acts as an “intensifier” for anxiety (Taylor et al., 2007). Studies linking perfectionism to statistics anxiety (Onwuegbuzie & Daley, 1999; Walsh & Ugumba-Agwunobi, 2002), tended to focus on socially prescribed perfectionism—a useful measure, but not one that misses important features of perfectionism such as perseveration and concern over making mistakes. Prior research also tended to exclude important covariates. By using covariates, we can rule out alternative explanations for bivariate relationships that arise through potential confounds. Finally, the Statistics Anxiety Rating Scale (STARS; Cruise et al., 1985), is a burdensome measure with 51 items, which makes it difficult to incorporate into more rigorous methods such as daily diary research. Though it is not the primary focus of the study, we also tested the factor structure of a short-form version of the STARS.
Objectives
We examined self-efficacy, anxiety sensitivity, concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, and their relationship to math/statistics anxiety among post-secondary university students when controlling for degree programs, gender, and graduate student status. Our analysis plan and hypotheses were pre-registered (see Supplementary Materials).
H1. Self-efficacy will be negatively related to math/statistics anxiety. That is, as self-efficacy increases, math/statistics anxiety will decrease, and vice versa.
H2. Anxiety sensitivity will be positively related to math/statistics anxiety. That is, as anxiety sensitivity increases, math/statistics anxiety will also increase.
H3. Concern over mistakes and doubts about actions will be positively related to math/statistics anxiety. As perfectionism increases, math/statistics anxiety will also increase.
H4. These three predictions will remain when statistically controlling for degree type (Arts, Science & Other), gender, and student status (undergraduate vs. graduate).
Method
Power Analyses
Power analyses suggested that a sample size of 319 is sufficient to detect correlations of .20 or larger, assuming an alpha of .05 and 95% power. This effect size was estimated by averaging the relationships between all STARS subscales and facets of perfectionism across three studies (Comerchero & Fortugno, 2013; Onwuegbuzie & Daley, 1999; Walsh & Ugumba-Agwunobi, 2002). According to our preregistered plan, we decided a-priori to combine the SONA and Prolific samples, using sample as a covariate.
Participants
Participants were current undergraduate or graduate students recruited online. We collected data from two populations—undergraduate students at Dalhousie University through the SONA system (the online psychology participant pool), and graduate students from Prolific (an online survey company). Prolific participants sign up voluntarily for the website and it is advertised via word of mouth and social media. On January 13, 2022, there were 6768 graduate students in Prolific’s pool, and the top 3 majors were Psychology (12%), Engineering (11%) and Computer Science (7%).
There were no inclusion or exclusion criteria for the SONA sample other than being a current undergraduate student at Dalhousie University. For the Prolific sample, current graduate students were eligible to participate. The SONA sample was recruited using ads posted online through Dalhousie University’s Department of Psychology Participant Pool website. The Prolific sample was recruited using internal advertisement on the Prolific website. Only registered users on the Prolific website who met the screening criteria (current graduate students) could see the ad for the study.
Exclusions
Our initial sample consisted of 506 participants (N = 149 Prolific; N = 357 SONA). Ten participants were excluded from the Prolific sample due to ineligibility (i.e., were not graduate students), six SONA participants who provided no data were removed, and 37 participants from the SONA sample opted for “observer” status (i.e., participated, but did not allow their data to be used for research). To handle missing data, only complete cases were used (i.e., listwise deletion). After listwise deletion, an additional 6 participants were omitted, leaving our final sample at n = 447.
Sample Demographics
After exclusions, ages ranged from 16 years old to 53 years old (M = 22.0 years, SD = 4.5 years). Around 70.2% were in Science programs, 15.0% Arts, and 14.8% Other. About 66.6% were not currently enrolled in a university-level statistics class and 69.6% had university-level statistics class experience. Most participants were women (68.2%).1 Ethnicities were White (76.6%), Asian (9.2%), Middle Eastern (5.6%), Hispanic/Latinx (2.9%), Black (2.0%) and Other (3.8%). SONA and Prolific samples are heterogeneous populations, which necessitates including sample as a covariate.
Materials
Screening
Prolific users were administered a screening survey when they first signed up as a participant with the service. Among other things, the survey asked users “Are you currently a student?” (yes/no) and “Which level of education are you currently in?” Only participants who answered “yes” to the student status question and one of “Graduate degree (MA/MSc/MPhil/other)” or “Doctorate degree (PhD/other)” were considered eligible for our study. The SONA participant pool comprised current undergraduate psychology students.
Baseline Demographics
Participants answered a demographics questionnaire following consent, asking about age, ethnicity, statistics course experience, education level, and university program (i.e., arts, science, other2). We expected all SONA participants would be undergraduates and all Prolific participants would be graduate students, but this was added as a double-check of screening.
Self-Efficacy
We measured self-efficacy with the 8-item New General Self-Efficacy Scale (NGSE; Chen et al., 2001). Participants responded on scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) on a timeframe of “the past year.” This scale demonstrates good alpha reliabilities when administered then re-administered (α = .85-.86; Chen et al., 2001), good test-retest reliability coefficients (rs from .62–.65), and a unidimensional factor structure (Chen et al., 2001).
Anxiety Sensitivity
We measured anxiety sensitivity with the 18-item Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3; Taylor et al., 2007). Participants responded using a 5-point scale 1 (very little) to 5 (very much) on their feelings “in general.” The ASI-3 has a hierarchical factor structure, with one higher-order factor and three lower-order factors (physical, cognitive, & social concerns; Kemper et al., 2012). We used the global total score rather than the three subscale scores, because (a) our hypotheses did not differ by subscale and (b) we wanted to reduce the overall number of statistical tests, and thus the familywise error rate. Internal consistencies tend to be strong for the total scores (α = .92; Kemper et al., 2012).
Perfectionism
Perfectionism was measured with short-form subscales from the Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (FMPS; Frost et al., 1990). We examined only the concern over mistakes (9 items) and doubts about actions (4 items) subscales. Participants responded on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) on a response scale "over the past several years." Graham et al. (2010) provide evidence of their reliability and factorial validity.
Statistics Anxiety
The Statistics Anxiety Rating Scale (STARS; Cruise et al., 1985) was originally a 51-item questionnaire. To reduce participant burden, we used 16 of the 51 questionnaire items. We selected these items by examining the top two or three highest factor loadings on each subscale from validity data presented in Hanna et al. (2008). Participants answer items using a 5-point scale that measure statistics-related anxiety (Test and Class Anxiety, Interpretation Anxiety, and Fear of Asking for Help) and statistics-related feelings (Worth of Statistics, Fear of Statistics Teachers, and Computational Self-Concept). Each subscale has three items, except for the Fear of Asking for Help subscale, which has two items given low factor loadings (< .68; Hanna et al., 2008) and Computational Self-Concept, where one item (i.e., “I'm too slow in my thinking to get through statistics”) was accidentally omitted due to an administrative error. Participants indicate the amount of anxiety (from no anxiety to strong anxiety) they would experience in the situations included in the first three subscales, and to what extent they agree or disagree (from strongly agree to strongly disagree) in the remaining subscales. Items from our abbreviated STARS can be found in Table 1.
Table 1
Factor Loadings for Abbreviated STARS
Item | Worth | Test | Help | Self-Concept | Teachers | Interpretation |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
16. I’m never going to use statistics so why should I have to take it? | .93 | -.004 | -.01 | -.05 | .02 | .005 |
13. I don’t see why I have to fill my head with statistics. It will have no use in my career. | .87 | .04 | .02 | .06 | -.03 | -.04 |
9. I feel statistics is a waste. | .51 | -.11 | .002 | .11 | .07 | .17 |
4. Doing an examination in a statistics course. | -.01 | .87 | -.01 | .02 | .02 | -.01 |
5. Walking into the room to take a statistics test. | -.03 | .75 | .05 | -.08 | .009 | -.01 |
1. Studying for an examination in a statistics course. | .05 | .58 | .04 | .13 | -.008 | .18 |
2. Going to ask my statistics teacher for individual help with material I am having difficulty understanding. | .01 | .01 | 1.01 | -.02 | .02 | -.04 |
7. Asking one of my teachers for help in understanding a printout. | -.04 | -.02 | .75 | .06 | -.03 | .10 |
12. I don’t have enough brains to get through statistics. | -.02 | .03 | -.009 | .93 | .02 | -.02 |
11. I can’t even understand secondary school maths; how can I possibly do statistics? | .10 | -.03 | .05 | .74 | .04 | .04 |
15. Statistics teachers task so fast you cannot logically follow them. | -.02 | .02 | 0 | -.05 | .92 | .006 |
14. Statistics teachers speak a different language. | .04 | -.008 | .04 | .20 | .63 | 0 |
10. Statistics teachers are so abstract they seem inhuman. | .16 | -.06 | -.009 | .04 | .49 | .05 |
8. Trying to understand the statistical analyses described in the abstract of a journal. | -.003 | -.02 | .04 | -.03 | .003 | .85 |
6. Interpreting the meaning of a probability value once I have found it. | .04 | .14 | -.03 | .03 | .09 | .59 |
3. Doing the coursework for a statistics course. | -.02 | .26 | .04 | .27 | -.05 | .37 |
Note. “Worth” refers to worth of statistics; “Test” refers to test and class anxiety; “Self-Concept” refers to computational self-concept; “Help” refers to fear of asking for help; “Teachers” refers to fear of statistics teachers; “Interpretation” refers to interpretation anxiety.
Math Anxiety
We measured math anxiety with the Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale (AMAS; Hopko et al., 2003). The AMAS has 9 items, with participants indicating the amount of anxiety (from low anxiety to high anxiety) they would experience in the situations given. This scale demonstrates both good reliability for the global scale (α = .90) and good convergent/divergent validity (Hopko et al., 2003).3
Table 3
Bivariate and Adjusted Correlations Between Predictors and Outcomes
Predictors & Outcomes | ra (95% CIb) | pholmc | r*d (95% CI) | pholm |
---|---|---|---|---|
Self-Efficacy | ||||
Test and Class Anxiety | -0.15 [-0.24, -0.06] | .009** | -0.12 [-0.22, -0.03] | .093 |
Interpretation Anxiety | -0.20 [-0.29, -0.11] | < .001*** | -0.18 [-0.27, -0.09] | .002** |
Fear of Asking for Help | -0.28 [-0.37, -0.19] | < .001*** | -0.24 [-0.34, -0.15] | < .001*** |
Worth of Statistics | -0.12 [-0.21, -0.03] | .066 | -0.09 [-0.18, 0.01] | .340 |
Fear of Statistics Teachers | -0.16 [-0.25, -0.07] | .006** | -0.13 [-0.22, -0.03] | .086 |
Computational Self-Concept | -0.29 [-0.37, -0.20] | < .001*** | -0.24 [-0.34, -0.15] | < .001*** |
Math Anxiety | -0.23 [-0.31, -0.14] | < .001*** | -0.20 [-0.30, -0.10] | .001** |
Anxiety Sensitivity | ||||
Test and Class Anxiety | 0.24 [0.15, 0.33] | < .001*** | 0.24 [0.15, 0.32] | < .001*** |
Interpretation Anxiety | 0.27 [0.18, 0.35] | < .001*** | 0.27 [0.18, 0.35] | < .001*** |
Fear of Asking for Help | 0.32 [0.23, 0.40] | < .001*** | 0.29 [0.21, 0.37] | < .001*** |
Worth of Statistics | 0.11 [0.02, 0.20] | .080 | 0.11 [0.01, 0.21] | .263 |
Fear of Statistics Teachers | 0.15 [0.06, 0.24] | .009** | 0.14 [0.04, 0.23] | .048* |
Computational Self-Concept | 0.25 [0.16, 0.34] | < .001*** | 0.24 [0.15, 0.33] | < .001*** |
Math Anxiety | 0.35 [0.26, 0.43] | < .001*** | 0.35 [0.26, 0.44] | < .001*** |
Concern Over Mistakes | ||||
Test and Class Anxiety | 0.28 [0.19, 0.36] | < .001*** | 0.23 [0.14, 0.32] | < .001*** |
Interpretation Anxiety | 0.28 [0.19, 0.36] | < .001*** | 0.25 [0.15, 0.34] | < .001*** |
Fear of Asking for Help | 0.40 [0.32, 0.47] | < .001*** | 0.36 [0.27, 0.44] | < .001*** |
Worth of Statistics | 0.06 [-0.03, 0.15] | .388 | 0.05 [-0.04, 0.14] | .494 |
Fear of Statistics Teachers | 0.10 [0.01, 0.19] | .133 | 0.08 [-0.02, 0.17] | .480 |
Computational Self-Concept | 0.20 [0.11, 0.28] | < .001*** | 0.18 [0.09, 0.27] | .001** |
Math Anxiety | 0.38 [0.29, 0.45] | < .001*** | 0.35 [0.26, 0.43] | < .001*** |
Doubts About Actions | ||||
Test and Class Anxiety | 0.25 [0.16, 0.34] | < .001*** | 0.23 [0.14, 0.33] | < .001*** |
Interpretation Anxiety | 0.26 [0.17, 0.34] | < .001*** | 0.24 [0.15, 0.33] | < .001*** |
Fear of Asking for Help | 0.31 [0.22, 0.39] | < .001*** | 0.28 [0.19, 0.37] | < .001*** |
Worth of Statistics | 0.01 [-0.08, 0.11] | .728 | -0.01 [-0.09, 0.08] | .832 |
Fear of Statistics Teachers | 0.10 [0.01, 0.19] | .133 | 0.07 [-0.02, 0.16] | .390 |
Computational Self-Concept | 0.22 [0.13, 0.30] | < .001*** | 0.18 [0.10, 0.27] | .001** |
Math Anxiety | 0.36 [0.28, 0.44] | < .001*** | 0.35 [0.26, 0.43] | < .001*** |
aPearson correlation coefficient. bConfidence interval. cp-value with Holm-Bonferroni correction. dStandardized regression coefficient after adjusting for covariates.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Procedure
The study was reviewed by Dalhousie University's Social Sciences and Humanities Research Ethics Board (#2020-5290). Participants were compensated with 0.5 credit points towards an eligible course (SONA) or £2.50 (Prolific). SONA participants could indicate “observer” status, meaning they may complete the study and receive compensation, but we would destroy their data and not use it for research. All questions had a “prefer not to answer” option, which was counted as missing data. After reading a consent form, participants took part in a cross-sectional survey using SurveyMonkey, where they answered each of the questionnaires specified above. Completing questionnaires took ~20 minutes. Upon completion, participants were debriefed and compensated.
Transparency, Openness, and Reproducibility
Our confirmatory hypotheses and data analyses were pre-registered, with a time-stamped plan located in the Pre-registration. The cleaned data in .csv format are openly available in the Supplementary Materials. The raw data contains sensitive personal information, such as IP addresses; thus, this data cannot be made publicly available due to ethical concerns. There are a few other measures (two perfectionism measures; demographics) included that did not involve pre-registered hypotheses and are not presented in this paper. A full list of item wording for all measures can be found in the codebook: https://osf.io/zh5ab
Scripts used for our primary data analyses can be located in the Supplementary Materials in the “Core Data Analysis” folder. Users must install the statistical analysis software R (https://www.r-project.org/) to run these scripts. Supplementary analyses and data visualization are located in the Supplementary Materials in the “Supplementary Analysis” folder. These data have not been published elsewhere outside of an honours thesis by the 1st author.
Results
Exploratory Factor Analysis
We conducted an exploratory factor analysis on the abbreviated STARS. We used maximum likelihood estimation for the extraction of factors, oblimin rotation4, and parallel analysis to select the number of factors. Table 1 below shows the factor loadings.
Parallel analysis suggested six factors; according to our preregistered plan, this was the decision criterion for determining the number of factors to retain. The abbreviated STARS follows the six-factor structure of Cruise et al.’s (1985) original scale. Six factors accounted for ~66.8% of the variance, with factors contributing to the cumulative variance explained as follows: Worth of statistics (12.81%), test and class anxiety (12.15%) computational self-concept (11.4%), fear of asking for help (10.49%), fear of statistics teachers (10.34%), and interpretation anxiety (9.64%). Examining model fit indices also suggested that the 6-factor model fit best (Table 2). Factor loadings above .40 were considered substantial (Stevens, 1992). Each item surpassed the .40 threshold except Item 3, although we deemed it close enough (.37) to the threshold to be worth keeping.
Table 2
Fit Indices for Nested Exploratory Factor Analysis Models
Model | RMSEA | TLI |
---|---|---|
1 Factor | 0.19 | 0.44 |
2 Factor | 0.15 | 0.64 |
3 Factor | 0.71 | 0.14 |
4 Factor | 0.82 | 0.11 |
5 Factor | 0.08 | 0.90 |
6 Factor | 0.04 | 0.97 |
Descriptives
Figure 1 below presents plots of the raw data with means indicated as dotted red lines for averaged total scores. Given that all measures could theoretically range from 1–5, most means were towards the middle of the possible response scale. Some notable exceptions are self-efficacy, which tended to be high for most participants, and the attitude factors of statistics anxiety, which were more infrequently endorsed. The density plots often approximated normal distributions, though the three "attitudes" factors for statistics anxiety tended to be positively skewed and the test and class anxiety was negatively skewed.
Figure 1
Density Plots, Means, and Cronbach’s Alpha Values
Confirmatory Hypothesis Tests
We calculated Pearson correlations between our predictors and math/statistics anxiety measures. Afterward, we controlled for gender, university program, and education level/sample (i.e., SONA/undergraduate vs. Prolific/graduate) by including these three variables as covariates in multiple regression. We adjusted p-values for multiple comparisons using a sequential Holm-Bonferroni correction (Holm, 1979) to adjust for familywise error rates. This adjustment was done on the 28 Pearson correlations (H1–H3) and for the 28 regression coefficients (H4) separately, rather than on all 56 tests. Table 3 below outlines our pre-registered confirmatory hypothesis tests. Throughout all analyses, confidence interval widths were approximately ±0.18, so magnitudes of effect sizes should be interpreted with this margin of error.
Prior to adjustment for covariates, self-efficacy was significantly negatively correlated with math anxiety and all statistics anxiety subscales except for worth of statistics. After controlling for gender, university program, and education level/sample (i.e., SONA vs. Prolific), self-efficacy’s relationships became non-significant with test and class anxiety (pholm = .093) and fear of statistics teachers (pholm = .086); however, the other relationships remained statistically significant with mostly similar effect sizes. Effect sizes ranged from -.12 to -.29 in bivariate correlations and from -.09 to -.24 after adding covariates.
Anxiety sensitivity was positively correlated with math anxiety and each statistics anxiety subscale except for worth of statistics (pholm = .08). Relationships remained statistically significant after controlling for gender, university program, and education level. Effect sizes ranged from .11 to .35 in both the bivariate correlations and after adding covariates.
The pattern for both perfectionism variables was similar. Both concern over mistakes and doubts about actions were positively correlated with math anxiety and 4 of 6 statistics anxiety subscales before and after adding covariates. Standardized regression coefficients adjusting for covariates ranged from .05 to .36 for concern over mistakes and from -.01 to .35 for doubts about actions.
Exploratory Analyses
Supplementary relationships between covariates and math/statistics anxiety and data visualizations are located in the Supplementary Materials. Broadly, these show that gender (i.e., being a woman) was positively associated with math/statistics anxiety (significant rs from .10–.26) and education level (i.e., being a graduate student) was negatively related to math/statistics anxiety (significant rs from -.13 to -.25). When looking at university program, we generally found that arts students had more statistics/math anxiety than other students. We present exploratory correlations when anxiety sensitivity is split into 3 subscales (physical, cognitive, and social concerns) in the online supplementary materials (Table S1). These results were virtually identical to the results with the total score.
Discussion
We generally found support for our hypotheses: Self-efficacy, anxiety sensitivity, and perfectionism were correlated with both statistics/math anxiety. The strongest relationships tended to be with items conceptually related to anxiety. The weakest relationships tended to be with the worth of statistics and fear of statistics subscales. These subscales tended to include items that lacked an anxious affect component (e.g., I feel statistics is a waste), so these personality facets may contribute primarily to heightened anxiety, but not necessarily to negative attitudes about statistics as a discipline.
Effect sizes for self-efficacy’s significant relationships to math/statistics anxiety ranged from -.09 to -.24 after covariate adjustment, and there were non-significant associations with the test and class anxiety, worth of statistics, and fear of statistics teachers subscales. Self-efficacy may correlate more strongly with depression than with anxiety or attitudes towards statistics. Interventions that focus on developing and maintaining self-efficacy might modestly protect against at-risk students’ math/statistics anxiety, but the small relationships suggest relying on self-efficacy interventions alone would be insufficient.
As in many other domains (Taylor et al., 2007), anxiety sensitivity was positively associated with statistics anxiety. When students experience anxiety in the classroom (e.g., a difficult exam), over-focus on the physical, social, and cognitive features of their own anxiety can send students into a spiral of increasing anxiety. For such students, it may help to teach simple re-focusing techniques such as box breathing (i.e., slow deliberate breathing strategies to reduce anxious arousal).
The two dimensions of self-critical perfectionism we measured (i.e., doubts about actions, concern over mistakes) showed a similar pattern. Both dimensions positively correlated with math anxiety, test and class anxiety, interpretation anxiety, and fear of asking for help and computational self-concept but generally did not associate with the factors more closely associated with attitudes towards statistics (i.e., the remaining two STARS factors). Self-critical perfectionists may experience anxiety towards statistics, but not necessarily negative feelings towards statistics as a discipline. The correlations with statistics anxiety are consistent with prior studies (Comerchero & Fortugno, 2013; Onwuegbuzie & Daley, 1999; Walsh & Ugumba-Agwunobi, 2002), and we expand on that evidence by establishing that these associations persist when accounting for gender, university program, and education level. Assisting students with perfectionistic tendencies might lead to reduced math anxiety and fear of statistics-related tasks, which could improve math/statistics performance.
None of our predictors were significantly correlated with the worth of statistics subscale. Though anxiety sensitivity was the only predictor we examined that was significantly related to fear of statistics teachers after covariate adjustment, its effect size was very close in magnitude to self-efficacy (-.13 vs. .14). Thus, the results for this measure provide only weak evidence of a relationship. The item wording for the fear of statistics teachers subscale focuses on perceptions of how confusing instructors are, rather than fear, so the weak evidence may be because this subscale lacks content validity.
Exploratory analyses suggest that students in arts programs have higher statistics anxiety, consistent with humanities students having a higher aversion towards math than science students (O’Leary et al., 2017). Women had slightly higher levels of statistics anxiety than men, consistent with research on clinical anxiety symptoms (Remes et al., 2016). Though we initially thought graduate students would have higher levels of statistics anxiety than undergraduates given higher rates of mental illness (Evans et al., 2018), we instead found that graduate students had lower levels of statistics anxiety. This could be due to greater experience, higher average cognitive ability, or simply an artifact of a more culturally diverse sample collected through Prolific. Collectively, these findings call to attention the need for educators to be mindful of equity and individual differences in student populations.
Constraints on Generality
These results should reasonably generalize to other university students in comparable universities with undergraduate or graduate programs; however, it should be noted that these samples are not representative nor randomly sampled. Procedures are straightforward, and unlikely to vary if changed from online to pen-and-paper. We do not necessarily expect generalization to other perfectionism subscales (e.g., other-oriented perfectionism). We do not expect results for self-efficacy to apply across every type of domain-specific self-efficacy (e.g., physical self-efficacy). However, results do appear to generalize across two quantitative domains (math and statistics). Results are likely contingent on the content of introductory statistics curriculums at this point in history.
Within McAdams and Pals’ (2006) framework, perfectionism, anxiety sensitivity and self-efficacy might be thought of as dispositional traits—broad, stable characteristics that cut across multiple domains. In contrast, statistics/math anxiety are characteristic adaptations—contextualized within a specific time, place and social role. An important constraint on the generality of these results is that we do not measure domain-specific variations of personality (e.g., measures modified to be statistics-classroom specific) nor do we measure generalized trait anxiety or neuroticism. Without measurement of personality and anxiety at multiple levels (e.g., traits and characteristic adaptations) it is more difficult to disentangle the domain-specific and generalized relationships between variables, though we expect that statistics anxiety is distinct from generalized trait anxiety (Papousek et al., 2012). The largest relationships are likely when all measures are domain-specific.
Limitations
Further research on math/statistics anxiety protective and risk factors with a broader undergraduate sample can add to this study’s findings. The cross-sectional design of our study means we are examining behaviours and feelings at a specific time. Thus, findings may not represent individuals’ true feelings over time. Conducting cross-sectional correlational analyses, while able to establish the presence of relationships, means we cannot infer causality. Future research might use longitudinal methods.
While our study provides useful psychometric data for an abbreviated version of the STARS, we inadvertently excluded an item on the computational self-concept subscale because of an administrative error. With only two items, findings for computational self-concept are less trustworthy. Selecting the top three factor loadings from Hanna et al. (2008) is a somewhat arbitrary selection criteria for item inclusion in a short form. Future research might apply statistical methods to test for pairwise differences between factor loadings to select items for the short form in a less arbitrary way. In addition, some elements of the STARS may be considered outdated (e.g., specifying anxiety towards statistics printouts), and future studies could explore developing a more contemporary version of the scale to possibly improve external validity. Future research might also investigate whether the lower levels of statistics anxiety in graduate student samples are due to higher average cognitive ability, graduate school selection processes, or merely an artifact of a more culturally diverse graduate student sample. Finally, Prolific uses an online crowdsourcing approach. Potential biases include (a) rapid-responder biases, since study slots are filled first-come first-serve; (b) sampling bias towards young, educated women; and (c) selection biases, since studies must compete for attention with a list of other studies which could be differentially appealing (Prolific, 2022).
Conclusion
Not all students are the same—they differ in experience, demographic characteristics, and personality. We found that individual differences in students predicted variation in statistics/math anxiety. Self-efficacy was modestly correlated with anxiety in the statistics classroom; as students’ self-confidence increases, their anxiety tends to decrease. Anxiety sensitivity intensified statistics anxiety through over-focus on the physical, cognitive, and social symptoms of anxiety. The perfectionism facets we studied focus on rigid black and white thinking (concern over mistakes) and perseveration (doubts about actions). Statistics education inevitably results in mistakes and uncertainty—which can be intolerable for perfectionistic people, resulting in greater anxiety. Demographically, women who are undergraduates in arts programs tended to experience the most fear in statistics classrooms. Nonetheless, the results for personality also held after controlling for gender, program, and graduate student status. Overall, our research suggests the importance of considering the unique personality and individual differences of our student population when teaching.